Skip to main content

The Motor Examination

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 1733 Accesses

Part of the book series: SpringerBriefs in Neuroscience ((TVOBTP))

Abstract

Because of the nature of development, with its dependence on sensorimotor interaction, an assessment of motor functioning is critical to a meaningful evaluation of the cognitive control system. In this regard, a motor examination should be considered critical to neuropsychological evaluation, particularly within the developing pediatric population since cognition is grounded sensorimotor behavior. Results of motor examinations, as reported above, are predictive of later cognitive control. Motor examinations should be systematic, and unfortunately, most commercially available neuropsychological assessment tools do not include systematic motor assessment. Many commercially available exams are multiple-component subtests that are organized idiosyncratically, instead of respecting the way in which motor behavior is organized within the human brain. Certain existing motor exams typically focus on aspects of unimanual and bimanual motor programming tasks which are clearly extremely useful. Abilities to perform these types of tasks emerge at around the age of 5 years, while the motor system stabilizes, with adult levels of performance expected sometime between the ages of 10 and 12 years [183, 290]. These tasks are difficult to norm, as inter-rater reliability can be difficult to achieve (Goldberg and Podell, independent personal communications, circa 2007). However, as a general rule, the three brain regions participate in motor activity in different ways, and this has potential localizing significance. Programming motor sequences is the property of frontal lobes [64, 165]. Intention programs, such as starting, perseverating, and stopping behaviors, as well as the lack of inhibition over voluntary movement, is basal ganglia governed [111, 195, 291]. Managing the coordination, or the rate, rhythm, and force of movements, in other words, the quality of movement, is under cerebellar control. In this regard, qualitative observations can be extremely important in interpreting the data obtained from motor examinations. A neurologist routinely relies upon this type of information. Why can’t a neuropsychologist do the same?

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  1. Banich, M.T. and R.J. Compton, Cognitive neuroscience. 3rd ed. 2011, Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. xxiii, 595 p.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Heilman, K.M. and L.G. Rothi, Apraxia in Clinical neuropsychology, K.M. Heilman and E. Valenstein, Editors. 2003, Oxford University Press: Oxford; New York. p. 215-235.

    Google Scholar 

  3. Kolb, B. and I.Q. Whishaw, Fundamentals of human neuropsychology. 6th ed. 2009, New York, NY: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Njiokiktjien, C., Developmental dyspraxias: Assessment and differential diagnosis., in Brain lesion localization and developmental functions, D. Riva and C. Njiokiktjien, Editors. 2010, John Libbey Eurotext: Montrouge, France. p. 157-86.

    Google Scholar 

  5. Blumenfeld, H., Neuroanatomy through clinical cases. 2nd ed. 2010, Sunderland, Mass.: Sinauer Associates. xxiii, 1006 p.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Welsh, M.C., B.F. Pennington, and D.B. Groisser, A normative‐developmental study of executive function: A window on prefrontal function in children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 1991. 7(2): p. 131-149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Rothi, L.J. and K.M. Heilman, Apraxia: the neuropsychology of action. Brain damage, behaviour and cognition series. 1997, Hove: Psychology. viii, 312 p.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Denckla, M.B., Revised Neurological Examination for Subtle Signs (1985). Psychopharmacol Bull, 1985. 21(4): p. 773-800.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Zhao, Q., et al., Neurological soft signs are not “soft” in brain structure and functional networks: evidence from ALE meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull, 2013.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Koziol, L.F. (2014). The Motor Examination. In: The Myth of Executive Functioning. SpringerBriefs in Neuroscience(). Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-04477-4_28

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics