Advertisement

Does Meditation Give Us Unique Insight into Ultimate Reality? The Ethical Aim of Buddhism

  • Hoyt L. EdgeEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Studies in Neuroscience, Consciousness and Spirituality book series (SNCS, volume 2)

Abstract

The first part of the paper focuses on the narrow philosophical question of whether one can know ultimate reality through meditation. I argue that such knowledge is not possible, that experience is always mediated. Interpreting the Buddha naturalistically, I argue that his aim was less about knowledge and more about providing insight into how one can live a flourishing life. In the second section of the paper, I discuss ways in which a philosophical approach (broadly understood) may benefit a team engaging in neurological studies of meditation. Such an approach might help in linguistic analysis, as well as in understanding the cross-cultural context of traditional meditation traditions. I further suggest that mindfulness might make one more open to scientific discovery. Finally, I propose that the study of meditation will both benefit and be benefited by theories of extended cognition.

Keywords

Altered State Mystical Experience Ultimate Reality Philosophical Approach Meditative Experience 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Aristotle. 1962. Nichomachian ethics. Trans. Oswald, Martin. Indianapolis: The Bobbs-Merrill Co.Google Scholar
  2. Austin, J.H. 2006. Zen-brain reflections: Reviewing recent developments in meditation and states of consciousness. Boston: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  3. Batchelor, S. 1997. Buddhism without beliefs. New York: Riverhead Books.Google Scholar
  4. Batchelor, S. 2010. Confessions of a Buddhist atheist. New York: Spiegel & Grau.Google Scholar
  5. Cash, M. forthcoming. Cognition without borders: “Third wave” socially distributed cognition and relational autonomy. Cognitive Systems Research. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2013.03.007
  6. Clark, A. 1997. Being here: Putting brain, body and world together again. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. A Bradford Book.Google Scholar
  7. Clark, A. 2008. Supersizing the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Collins, S. 1982. Selfless persons: Imagery and thought in Theravada Buddhism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Deshmukt, V.D. 2006. Neurscience of meditation. TSW Holistic Health and Medicine 1: 275–289. doi: 10.1100/tswhhm.2006.244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Ellwood Jr., R.S. 1980. Mysticism and religion. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  11. Flanagan, O. 2011. The Bodhisatva’s brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  12. Gombrich, R. 2009. What the Buddha thought. London: Equinox Publishing.Google Scholar
  13. Greenberg, J., K. Reiner, and N. Meiran. 2012. “Mind the trap”: Mindfulness practice reduces cognitive rigidity. PLoS ONE 7(5): e36206. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036206.PubMedCentralPubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hanson, R.N. 1958. Patterns of discovery. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Heidegger, M. 1963. Sein und Zeit. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag.Google Scholar
  16. Hume, D. 1988. An enquiry concerning human understanding. LaSalle: Open Court.Google Scholar
  17. Husserl, E. 1973. Logical investigations. Trans. J.N. Findlay. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  18. James, W. 1902. The varieties of religious experience: A study in human nature. London: Longmans, Green.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kuhn, T. 1962. The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  20. London Buddhist Vihara. http://www.londonbuddhistvihara.org/med_aware/pmedinsight.htm. Accessed 10 Oct 2012.
  21. Lutz, A., J.D. Dunne, and R.J. Davidson. 2007. Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: an introduction. In Cambridge handbook of consciousness, ed. P. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson, 499–554. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Markus, H.R., and S. Kitayama. 1991. Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review 98: 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Marsella, A. 1985. A category of the human mind: The notion of the person; the notion of the self. In Culture and self: Asian and American perspectives, ed. Anthony Marsella, George DeVos, and Francis Hsu, 281–308. New York: Travistock Publiscations.Google Scholar
  24. Meditation. http://www.buddhanet.net/cmdsg/getting8.htm. Accessed 10 Oct 2012.
  25. Merleau-Ponty, M. 1962. Phenomenology of perception. Trans. Colin Smith. New York: Humanities Press.Google Scholar
  26. Morris, B. 1994. Anthropology of the self. Boulder: Pluto Press.Google Scholar
  27. Nietzsche, F. 1960. Joyful wisdom. Trans. Common, Thomas. New York: Fredrick Ungar.Google Scholar
  28. Nisbett, R. 2003. The geography of thought. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  29. Nisbett, R.E., and Y. Miyamoto. 2005. The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Science 9: 467–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Polanyi, M. 1966. The tacit dimension. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  31. Rorty, R. 1979. Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Rosemont Jr., H. 1991. Rights-bearing individuals and role-bearing persons. In Rules, rituals, and responsibilities, ed. Mary I. Bockover, 71–101. LaSalle: Open Court.Google Scholar
  33. Schmidt, S. 2011. Mindfulness in east and west – is it the same? In Neuroscience, consciousness and spirituality, ed. H. Walach, S. Schmidt, and W.B. Jonas, 23–38. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Stace, W.T. 1972. Mysticism and philosophy. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  35. Suzuki, D.T. 1972. What is Zen. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  36. Suzuki, D.T. 1977. An introduction to Zen Buddhism. New York: Grove Press.Google Scholar
  37. Sweder, R.A., and E.J. Bourne. 1984. Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In Culture theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion, ed. R.A. Schweder and R.A. Levine, 158–199. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Tart, C.T. 1975. Science, states of consciousness, and spiritual experiences: The need for state-specific sciences. In Transpersonal psychologies, ed. C. Tart, 11–58. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  39. Underhill, E. 1967. Mysticism: A study in the nature and development of man’s spiritual consciousness. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  40. Weinberg, J., S. Nichols, and S. Stich. 2008. Normativity and epistemic intuitions. In Experimental philosophy, ed. J. Knobe and S. Nichols, 17–47. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Philosophy and ReligionRollins CollegeWinter ParkUSA

Personalised recommendations