Skip to main content

Meditation and Hypnosis at the Intersection Between Phenomenology and Cognitive Science

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Meditation – Neuroscientific Approaches and Philosophical Implications

Abstract

Cognitive scientists increasingly turn to contemplative practices such as hypnosis and meditation to shed light on consciousness and cognition. By their very nature, such practices call scientists to address the qualitative, lived experience of the subject. Yet, while the rise of contemplative techniques in neuroscience research has highlighted the importance of incorporating subjective experience within the empirical sciences of mind, the practical reality of marrying first- and third-person methods remains largely unactualised. Given that hypnosis and meditation exert powerful influence on subjective experience, we propose that they can serve as potent instruments for elucidating the structures and mechanisms of conscious experience in cognitive science settings. Here we discuss the motivation for a so-called ‘neurophenomonological’ approach and outline recent findings from the domains of hypnosis and meditation. Concrete examples illustrate how such contemplative practices can go beyond their place as objects of investigation to emerge as complementary experimental tools, thereby advancing the synthesis of scientific and phenomenological studies of mind (This article draws on ideas and expositions that ML and AR authored in the introduction of a 2012 special issue on hypnosis and meditation in The Journal of Mind-Body Regulation (see volume 2, issue 1)).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Allen, M., M. Dietz, K.S. Blair, M. van Beek, G. Rees, P. Vestergaard-Poulsen, et al. 2012. Cognitive-affective neural plasticity following active-controlled mindfulness intervention. The Journal of Neuroscience 32(44): 15601–15610.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Augustinova, M., and L. Ferrand. 2012. Suggestion does not de-automatize word reading: Evidence from the semantically based Stroop task. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 19(3): 521–527. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0217-y.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandettini, P.A. 2012. Twenty years of functional MRI: The science and the stories. NeuroImage 62(2): 575–588. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.04.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barnier, A.J., R.E. Cox, A. O’Connor, M. Coltheart, R. Langdon, N. Breen, et al. 2008. Developing hypnotic analogues of clinical delusions: mirrored-self misidentification. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 13(5): 406–430. doi:10.1080/13546800802355666.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blakemore, S.J., D.A. Oakley, and C.D. Frith. 2003. Delusions of alien control in the normal brain. Neuropsychologia 41(8): 1058–1067. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(02)00313-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brefczynski-Lewis, J.A., A. Lutz, H.S. Schaefer, D.B. Levinson, and R.J. Davidson. 2007. Neural correlates of attentional expertise in long-term meditation practitioners. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104(27): 11483–11488. doi:10.1073/pnas.0606552104.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, J.A., P.D. Worhunsky, J.R. Gray, Y.-Y. Tang, J. Weber, and H. Kober. 2011. Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. doi:10.1073/pnas.1112029108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckner, R.L., J.R. Andrews‐Hanna, and D.L. Schacter. 2008. The brain’s default network. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1124(1): 1–38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Callard, F., J. Smallwood, and D.S. Margulies. 2012. Default positions: How neuroscience’s historical legacy has hampered investigation of the resting mind. Frontiers in Psychology 3: 321.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cardeña, E., P. Jönsson, D.B. Terhune, and D. Marcusson-Clavertz. 2012. The neurophenomenology of neutral hypnosis. Cortex 49(2): 375–385. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.04.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Casiglia, E., P.S. Schiff, E. Facco, A. Gabbana, V. Tikhonoff, L. Schiavon, et al. 2010. Neurophysiological correlates of post-hypnotic alexia: A controlled study with Stroop test. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis 52(3): 219–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Christoff, K., D. Cosmelli, D. Legrand, and E. Thompson. 2011. Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(3): 104–112. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen Kadosh, R., A. Henik, A. Catena, V. Walsh, and L.J. Fuentes. 2009. Induced cross-modal synaesthetic experience without abnormal neuronal connections. Psychological Science 20(2): 258–265. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02286.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deeley, Q., D.A. Oakley, B. Toone, V. Giampietro, M.J. Brammer, S.C.R. Williams, et al. 2012. Modulating the default mode network using hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 60(2): 206–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Depraz, N., F.J. Varela, and P. Vermersch. 2000. The gesture of awareness: An account of its structural dynamics. In Investigating phenomenal consciousness: New methodologies and maps, Advances in consciousness research, vol. 13, ed. M. Velmans, 121–136. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Depraz, N., F.J. Varela, and P. Vermersch. 2003. On becoming aware: A pragmatics of experiencing. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Farb, N.A.S., Z.V. Segal, H. Mayberg, J. Bean, D. McKeon, Z. Fatima, et al. 2007. Attending to the present: Mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2(4): 313–322. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm030.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, K.C.R., P. Zakarauskas, M. Dixon, M. Ellamil, E. Thompson, and K. Christoff. 2012. Meditation experience predicts introspective accuracy. PLoS One 7(9): e45370. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0045370.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. 2000. Philosophical conceptions of the self: Implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4(1): 14–21. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01417-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., and J. Brøsted Sørensen. 2006. Experimenting with phenomenology. Consciousness and Cognition 15(1): 119–134. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.03.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gard, T., B.K. Hölzel, A.T. Sack, H. Hempel, S.W. Lazar, D. Vaitl, et al. 2012. Pain attenuation through mindfulness is associated with decreased cognitive control and increased sensory processing in the brain. Cerebral Cortex 22(11): 2692–2702. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr352.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, J.A., J. Courtemanche, and P. Rainville. 2011. A non-elaborative mental stance and decoupling of executive and pain-related cortices predicts low pain sensitivity in Zen meditators. Pain 152(1): 150–156. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasenkamp, W., C.D. Wilson-Mendenhall, E. Duncan, and L.W. Barsalou. 2012. Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: A fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. NeuroImage 59(1): 750–760.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iani, C., F. Ricci, E. Gherri, and S. Rubichi. 2006. Hypnotic suggestion modulates cognitive conflict: The case of the flanker compatibility effect. Psychological Science 17(8): 721–727.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iani, C., F. Ricci, G. Baroni, and S. Rubichi. 2009. Attention control and susceptibility to hypnosis. Consciousness and Cognition 18(4): 856–863.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Khalsa, S.S., D. Rudrauf, A.R. Damasio, R.J. Davidson, A. Lutz, and D. Tranel. 2008. Interoceptive awareness in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology 45(4): 671–677. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00666.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kihlstrom, J.F. 2008. The domain of hypnosis, revisited. In Oxford handbook of hypnosis, ed. M. Nash and A. Barnier, 21–52. Oxford: Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsch, I., and W. Braffman. 2001. Imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability. Current Directions in Psychological Science 10(2): 57–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S.M., W.L. Thompson, M.F. Costantini-Ferrando, N.M. Alpert, and D. Spiegel. 2000. Hypnotic visual illusion alters color processing in the brain. The American Journal of Psychiatry 157(8): 1279–1284. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lifshitz, M., and A. Raz. 2012. Hypnosis and meditation: Vehicles of attention and suggestion. The Journal of Mind–Body Regulation 2(1): 3–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lifshitz, M., N. Aubert-Bonn, A. Fischer, I.F. Kashem, and A. Raz. 2013. Using suggestion to modulate automatic processes: From Stroop to McGurk and beyond. Cortex 49(2): 463–473. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.08.007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A. 2004. Introduction—the explanatory gap: To close or to bridge? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 3(4): 325–330. doi:10.1023/b:phen.0000048938.77185.44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., and E. Thompson. 2003. Neurophenomenology – integrating subjective experience and brain dynamics in the neuroscience of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 10(9–10): 31–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., J.D. Dunne, and R.J. Davidson. 2006. Meditation and the neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In Cambridge handbook of consciousness, ed. P.D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, and E. Thompson. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutz, A., H.A. Slagter, J.D. Dunne, and R.J. Davidson. 2008. Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12(4): 163–169.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, S.J., L.S. Das, N.H. Michael, and J.C. Williams. 2006. Mindfulness, acceptance, and hypnosis: Cognitive and clinical perspectives. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 54(2): 143–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, S.J., S. Barnes, A. Deming, and M. Accardi. 2010. Hypnosis, rumination, and depression: Catalyzing attention and mindfulness-based treatments. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 58(2): 202–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lynn, S.J., A. Malaktaris, R. Maxwell, D.I. Mellinger, and D. van der Kloet. 2012. Do hypnosis and mindfulness practices inhabit a common domain? Implications for research, clinical practice, and forensic science. The Journal of Mind–Body Regulation 2(1): 12–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, K.A., E. Ferrer, S.R. Aichele, D.A. Bridwell, A.P. Zanesco, T.L. Jacobs, et al. 2010. Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science 21(6): 829.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C.M. 1991. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin 109(2): 163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C.M., and K. Dunbar. 1988. Training and Stroop-like interference: Evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 14(1): 126.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzoni, G., E. Rotriquenz, C. Carvalho, M. Vannucci, K. Roberts, and I. Kirsch. 2009. Suggested visual hallucinations in and out of hypnosis. Consciousness and Cognition 18(2): 494–499.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGeown, W.J., G. Mazzoni, A. Venneri, and I. Kirsch. 2009. Hypnotic induction decreases anterior default mode activity. Consciousness and Cognition 18(4): 848–855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGeown, W.J., A. Venneri, I. Kirsch, L. Nocetti, K. Roberts, L. Foan, et al. 2012. Suggested visual hallucination without hypnosis enhances activity in visual areas of the brain. Consciousness and Cognition 21(1): 100–116. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2011.10.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, A., T. Gruber, J. Derose, and P. Malinowski. 2012. Regular, brief mindfulness meditation practice improves electrophysiological markers of attentional control. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 18.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mrazek, M.D., J. Smallwood, and J.W. Schooler. 2012. Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion 12(3): 442.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R.E., and T.D. Wilson. 1977. Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes. Psychological Review 84(3): 231–259. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.84.3.231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oakley, D.A., and P.W. Halligan. 2009. Hypnotic suggestion and cognitive neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(6): 264–270.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oakley, D.A., and P.W. Halligan. 2010. Psychophysiological foundations of hypnosis and suggestion. In Handbook of clinical hypnosis, 2nd ed, ed. Jay Lynn Steven, Judith W. Rhue, and I. Kirsch. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pagnoni, G. 2012. Dynamical properties of BOLD activity from the ventral posteromedial cortex associated with meditation and attentional skills. The Journal of Neuroscience 32(15): 5242–5249.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pagnoni, G., M. Cekic, and Y. Guo. 2008. “Thinking about not-thinking”: Neural correlates of conceptual processing during Zen meditation. PLoS One 3(9): e3083.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parris, B.A., Z. Dienes, and T.L. Hodgson. 2012. Temporal constraints of the word blindness posthypnotic suggestion on Stroop task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 38(4): 833.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Priftis, K., S. Schiff, V. Tikhonoff, N. Giordano, P. Amodio, C. Umiltà, et al. 2011. Hypnosis meets neuropsychology: Simulating visuospatial neglect in healthy participants. Neuropsychologia 49(12): 3346–3350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.08.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pyka, M., M. Burgmer, T. Lenzen, R. Pioch, U. Dannlowski, B. Pfleiderer, et al. 2011. Brain correlates of hypnotic paralysis—a resting-state fMRI study. NeuroImage 56(4): 2173–2182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M.E. 2010. Two views of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(4): 180–190.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rainville, P., and D.D. Price. 2003. Hypnosis phenomenology and the neurobiology of consciousness. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis 51(2): 105–129. doi:10.1076/iceh.51.2.105.14613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., and J. Buhle. 2006. Typologies of attentional networks. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7(5): 367–379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., and N.K.J. Campbell. 2011. Can suggestion obviate reading? Supplementing primary Stroop evidence with exploratory negative priming analyses. Consciousness and Cognition 20(2): 312–320. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., T. Shapiro, J. Fan, and M.I. Posner. 2002. Hypnotic suggestion and the modulation of Stroop interference. Archives of General Psychiatry 59(12): 1155.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., K.S. Landzberg, H.R. Schweizer, Z.R. Zephrani, T. Shapiro, J. Fan, et al. 2003. Posthypnotic suggestion and the modulation of Stroop interference under cycloplegia. Consciousness and Cognition 12(3): 332–346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., J. Fan, and M.I. Posner. 2005. Hypnotic suggestion reduces conflict in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102(28): 9978–9983.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., I. Kirsch, J. Pollard, and Y. Nitkin-Kaner. 2006. Suggestion reduces the stroop effect. Psychological Science 17(2): 91–95. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01669.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raz, A., M. Moreno-Íniguez, L. Martin, and H. Zhu. 2007. Suggestion overrides the Stroop effect in highly hypnotizable individuals. Consciousness and Cognition 16(2): 331–338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P., and D. Zahavi. 2011. The uncanny mirror: A re-framing of mirror self-experience. Consciousness and Cognition 20(2): 204–213. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmicking, D. 2010. A toolbox of phenomenological methods. In Handbook of phenomenology and cognitive science, ed. S. Gallagher and D. Schmicking, 35–56. New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., and R.M. Shiffrin. 1977. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review 84(1): 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siderits, M., E. Thompson, and D. Zahavi (eds.). 2011. Self, no-self? Perspectives from analytical, phenomenological, and Indian traditions. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slagter, H.A., A. Lutz, L.L. Greischar, A.D. Francis, S. Nieuwenhuis, J.M. Davis, et al. 2007. Mental training affects distribution of limited brain resources. PLoS Biology 5(6): e138.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Slagter, H.A., R.J. Davidson, and A. Lutz. 2011. Mental training as a tool in the neuroscientific study of brain and cognitive plasticity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 17. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00017.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinbock, A.J. 2004. Affection and attention: On the phenomenology of becoming aware. Continental Philosophy Review 37(1): 21–43. doi:10.1023/B:MAWO.0000049298.44397.be.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroop, J.R. 1935. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology 18(6): 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, Y.Y., Y.H. Ma, J. Wang, Y.X. Fan, S.G. Feng, Q.L. Lu, et al. 2007. Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104: 17152–17156.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, V.A., J. Grant, V. Daneault, G. Scavone, E. Breton, S. Roffe-Vidal, et al. 2011. Impact of mindfulness on the neural responses to emotional pictures in experienced and beginner meditators. NeuroImage 57(4): 1524–1533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, V.A., V. Daneault, J. Grant, G. Scavone, E. Breton, S. Roffe-Vidal, et al. 2012. Impact of meditation training on the default mode network during a restful state. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 8(1): 4–14. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr087.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terhune, D.B., E. Cardeña, and M. Lindgren. 2010. Disruption of synaesthesia by posthypnotic suggestion: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia 48(11): 3360–3364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, S., W. Singer, and L. Melloni. 2012. Meditation increases the depth of information processing and improves the allocation of attention in space. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6: 133. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00133.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F.J. 1996. Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem. Journal of Consciousness Studies 3: 330–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F.J. 1998. A science of consciousness as if experience mattered. In Toward a science of consciousness II: The second Tucson discussions and debates, vol. 2, ed. S.R. Hameroff, A.W. Kaszniak, and A.C. Scott, 31–44. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F.J., E.T. Thompson, and E. Rosch. 1992. The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woody, E., and H. Szechtman. 2011. Using hypnosis to develop and test models of psychopathology. Mind-Body Regulation 1(1): 4–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeidan, F., K.T. Martucci, R.A. Kraft, N.S. Gordon, J.G. McHaffie, and R.C. Coghill. 2011. Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindfulness meditation. The Journal of Neuroscience 31(14): 5540–5548. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5791-10.2011.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amir Raz .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lifshitz, M., Cusumano, E.P., Raz, A. (2014). Meditation and Hypnosis at the Intersection Between Phenomenology and Cognitive Science. In: Schmidt, S., Walach, H. (eds) Meditation – Neuroscientific Approaches and Philosophical Implications. Studies in Neuroscience, Consciousness and Spirituality, vol 2. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-01634-4_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics