Skip to main content

Can you Learn to Love Grammar and so Make it Grow? On the Role of Affect in L2 Development

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Essential Topics in Applied Linguistics and Multilingualism

Part of the book series: Second Language Learning and Teaching ((SLLT))

Abstract

In the nineteen seventies, Burt and Dulay suggested that negative emotions might act as an input filter inhibiting grammatical development. This idea was reformulated by Krashen as the Affective Filter Hypothesis (AFH) (Krashen 1981, 1982). Educators, applied linguists and SLA researchers have all stressed the value of positive attitudes on learning success. Research on emotions and language learning has mostly focused on the lexicon, on individual styles of learning and rates of success (Dörnyei 2003; Dewaele 2005; Pavlenko 2005) rather than the acquisition of, specifically, syntax and phonology. These are areas which, unlike the lexicon, are generally held to become significantly more difficult with age (Singleton 1995 and 1999). However motivated older learners may be to develop their phonological and syntactic skills, the desired development is by no means guaranteed. How may this be explained in terms of the psycholinguistic mechanisms involved? The AFH was stated in very general terms and never really elaborated. Nevertheless, since the AFH was originally formulated, there has been a lot of research on affect in cognitive neuroscience (e.g. Damasio 1994, 1999; LeDoux 2002). To guide investigations into how affect variably influences grammatical, lexical, semantic and pragmatic growth, the AFH is in need of updating. The MOGUL framework, which takes account of recent research across a range of disciplines, will be used to elaborate it in finer detail in a first attempt to provide a better basis for empirical investigation (see also Sharwood Smith and Truscott 2013).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    This of course does hold so obviously for those who regard the acquisition of grammar as driven by exactly the same mechanisms as any other kind of learning but the affective mechanisms that facilitate or inhibit learning in general is still an area that needs a great deal more research.

  2. 2.

    In Krashen's terms this would be ‘i + 1’, the next grammatical morpheme to be acquired according to the Input Hypothesis (Krashen 1985).

  3. 3.

    This idea is commonly referred to as the James-Lange Theory.

  4. 4.

    Event Related Potential: the participants' brain electrical activity was monitored as they performed the tasks.

  5. 5.

    Thanks are due to Guillaume Thierry for helpful suggestions in interpreting these results.

  6. 6.

    Pienemann’s Processability theory incorporates processing concerns into accounts of language development by explaining stages of acquisition as being governed by the relative processability of linguistic constructions. This theory therefore proposes principles that explain differences between one observed developmental stage and the next. This is different from explaining what the precise mechanisms are that are in operation during the on-line processing of L2 at stage ‘s’ that affect a change into stage ‘s + 1’ (Pienemann 1998). Similarly VanPatten’s Input Processing theory outlines numerous principles that might explain how the learner’s attention is guided to specific aspects of the input. It is not meant to be about L2 parsing or how L2 parsing relates to changes in the learner grammar (VanPattten 2004).

  7. 7.

    Jackendoff's architecture can be seen in two ways, statically as a linguistic-theoretical construct, or in psycholinguistic terms, as a processing system. MOGUL opts for the second way. Chains are constructed incrementally and in parallel until the best available match is found.

  8. 8.

    Not always, however, with exactly the same implications (Sharwood Smith 2004).

  9. 9.

    In Sharwood Smith & Truscott (2013) the word 'extramodular' is used with the same meaning signifying 'outside the language module'.

  10. 10.

    Here, MOGUL appears to differ somewhat from Jackendoff's proposals for whom some basic phonological features may feature in awareness and give rise to the voice in the head (Jackendoff 1987: 291, 2002: 274).

  11. 11.

    Here I refer to interfaces, not as they appear in the MOGUL framework, but as discussed in Sharwood Smith (1981) and in much more elaborated form as Krashen's (strong/no) ‘interface hypothesis' (Krashen 1985). In MOGUL, interfaces have a more precise technical meaning.

  12. 12.

    More technically stated, “no interface exists between affective structures (AfS) and either phonological structures PS or syntactic structures (SS)”.

  13. 13.

    This states that older L2 acquirers no longer have access to domain-specific mechanisms constrained by Universal Grammar and must rely on general problem-solving skills (Bley-Vroman 1990).

  14. 14.

    As John Truscott (personal communication) has reminded me, one challenge to be faced is how to account, psycholinguistically, for responses in grammaticality judgment tests. If any effect of metalinguistic knowledge can be ruled out, how should we explain the processing of sentences that are judged ungrammatical? An intuitive rejection of an ungrammatical sentence implies negative valence. As with the P600 event-related potential, where is the most plausible location for this valence (within whatever theoretical framework has been selected to guide investigation)?

References

  • Alderfer, C. P. 1972. Existence, relatedness, and growth; human needs in organizational settings. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bley-Vroman R. 1990. The logical problem of foreign language learning. Linguistic Analysis 20: 3–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. 1994. Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. 1999. The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. 2000. A second chance for emotion. In Cognitive neuroscience of emotion, eds. R. D. Lane and L. Nadel, 12–23. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewaele J. -M. and A. Pavlenko. 2002. Emotional vocabulary in interlanguage. Language Learning 52: 263–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewaele, J. -M. 2005. Investigating the psychological and emotional dimensions in instructed language learning: Obstacles and possibilities. Modern Language Journal 89: 367–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewaele, J. -M. 2010. Emotions in multiple languages. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörnyei, Z. 2001. Teaching and researching motivation. Harlow: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörnyei, Z. 2003. Attitudes, orientations, and motivations in language learning: Advances in theory, research, and applications. In Attitudes, orientations and motivations in language learning, ed. Z. Dörnyei, 3–32. Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dörnyei, Z. and I. Ottó. 1998. Motivation in action: Process model of L2 motivation. Applied Linguistics 4: 43–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dulay, H., Burt, M. and S. Krashen. 1982. Language two. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dulay, H. and M. Burt. 1977. Remarks on creativity in language acquisition. In Viewpoints on English as a second language, eds. M. Burt, H. Dulay and M. Finnochiaro, 95–126. New York: Regents.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R. C. and W. E. Lambert. 1972. Attitudes and motivation in second-language learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopp, H. and M. Schmid. 2013. Perceived foreign accent in first language attrition and second language acquisition: The impact of age of acquisition and bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics 34: 361–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. 1979. Emotions as motivations: A evolutionary-developmental perspective. In Nebraska symposium on motivation, eds. H. E. Howe, Jr. and R. A. Dienstbier, 163–200. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. 1987. Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. 2002 Foundations of language: Brain, meaning, grammar, evolution. Oxford—New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jacobs, B. and J. Schumann. 1992. Language acquisition and the neurosciences: Towards a more integrative perspective. Applied Linguistics 13: 283–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. 1884. What is an emotion. Mind 9: 188–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. 1894. The physical basis of emotion. Psychological Review 1: 516–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. D. 1981. Aptitude and attitude in relation to second language acquisition and learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. D. 1982. Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. D. 1985. The input hypothesis: Issues and implications. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. D. and L. Nadel, eds. 2000. Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. 1996. The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeDoux, J. 2002. The synaptic self. Harmondsworth: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M. C. 1996. Self organising cognitive appraisals. Cognition and Emotion 10: 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, A. H. 1943. A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review 50: 370–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Öhman, A, A. Flykt and D. Lunquist. 2000. Unconscious emotion: Evolutionary perspectives, psychophysiological data and neuropsychological mechanisms. In Cognitive neuroscience of emotion, eds. R. D. Lane and L. Nadel, 296–327. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oxford, R. L. and J. Shearin. 1994. Language learning motivation: Expanding the theoretical framework. Modern Language Journal 78: 12–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oxford, R. L. 1996. Language learning motivation: Pathways to the new century. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pienemann, M. 1998. Language processing and second language development: Processability theory. Amsterdam—Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlenko, A. 2005. Emotions and multilingualism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulvermüller, F. and J. Schumann. 1994. Neurobiological mechanisms of language acquisition. Language Learning 44: 681–734.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, J. H. 1975. Affective factors and the problem of age in second language acquisition. Language Learning 25: 209–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, J. H. 1990. The role of the amygdala as a mediator of affect and cognition in second language acquisition. In Proceedings of the Georgetown University Round Table on Language and Linguistics, ed. J. E. Alatis, 169–176. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, J. H. 1997 The neurobiology of affect in language. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selinker, L. 1972 Interlanguage. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching 10: 209–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharwood Smith, M. 1981. Consciousness-raising and the second language learner. Applied Linguistics 2: 159–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharwood Smith, M. 2004. In two minds about grammar: On the interaction of linguistic and metalinguistic knowledge in performance. Transactions of the Philological Society 102: 255–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharwood Smith, M., J. Truscott and R. Hawkins. 2013. Explaining change in transition grammars. In A handbook of second language acquisition, eds. J. Herschensohn and M. Young-Scholten, 560–580. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sharwood Smith, M. and J. Truscott. 2013. The multilingual mind: A modular processing perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singleton, D. 1995. Introduction: A critical look at the critical period hypothesis is second language acquisition research. In The age factor in second language acquisition, eds. D. Singleton and Z. Lengyel, 1–29. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singleton, D. 1999. Exploring the second language mental lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thierry, G. and Y. J. Wu. 2007. Brain potentials reveal unconscious translation during foreign language comprehension. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA 104: 12530–15535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Truscott, J. and M. Sharwood Smith. 2011. Input, intake, and consciousness: The quest for a theoretical foundation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33: 297–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Truscott, J. 2013. The MOGUL framework for SLA. In The Routledge encyclopedia of second language acquisition, ed. P. Robinson, 435–436. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Truscott, J., in preparation. Consciousness and second language learning. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Truscott, J. and M. Sharwood Smith. 2004. Acquisition by processing: A modular perspective on language development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7: 1–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Lier, L. 1996. Interaction in the language curriculum: Awareness, autonomy, and authenticity. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • VanPatten. 2004. Input Processing in SLA. In Processing instruction: Theory, research, and commentary, ed. B. VanPatten, 1–31. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y. -J. and G. Thierry. 2012. How reading in a second language protects your heart. Journal of Neuroscience 32: 6485–6489.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Sharwood Smith .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer International Publishing Switzerland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Sharwood Smith, M. (2014). Can you Learn to Love Grammar and so Make it Grow? On the Role of Affect in L2 Development. In: Pawlak, M., Aronin, L. (eds) Essential Topics in Applied Linguistics and Multilingualism. Second Language Learning and Teaching. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-01414-2_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics