Skip to main content

When Law Changes Attitudes Within the Integrated Approach

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
When Do People Obey Laws?

Part of the book series: International Law and Economics ((ILEC))

  • 41 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter considers when and under what conditions will laws lead to attitudinal and behavioral change in the long run. First, it considers the conditions necessary for continued compliance through acquiescence, identification, and internalization. Second, it considers the conditions under which individuals who may have complied with the law through acquiescence (fear of legal sanction) or weak identification (fear of social sanctions) can be motivated to comply with laws voluntarily (through strong identification and/or internalization). In the process, it identifies the social and institutional conditions that societies and policymakers can focus on to create a general culture in favor of legal compliance. As importantly, it highlights the limits of using only laws and formal enforcement mechanisms for creating lasting attitudinal change. In fact, as the chapter discusses in the last sections, over-emphasizing the role of law and legal enforcement can have the perverse effect of undermining conditions necessary for future compliance with the law.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Roy (2022a, b) explains how initial compliance decisions can create cascades in favor of compliance.

  2. 2.

    As explained in Hogg (2003), some social identities such as ethnic groups or nationality are referred to as “ascribed” social identities since they are not chosen by the individual while others such as one’s political leanings and professional membership are considered as “achieved” social identities. However, the prominence of these identities, including ascribed social identities, can evolve.

  3. 3.

    Supra Chap. 2, 4950.

  4. 4.

    Supra Chap. 2, Sect. 3.2.

  5. 5.

    To this extent, continued compliance through identification can resemble compliance to social norms since both are contingent on social expectations about others’ behaviors and attitudes.

  6. 6.

    Similarly, if we were complying with a law presuming it will help us better coordinate or cooperate with other people then continued compliance will require that compliance with the law help achieve these instrumental goals.

  7. 7.

    The precise discussion on how we react to two contradictory influences, i.e., the state prohibiting the behavior and our social group prescribing it is considered more in detail in Chap. 4 where the book considers the interaction between law and social norms in shaping behaviors.

  8. 8.

    Leon Festinger is considered as one of the most influential psychologists of his time for developing this concept of cognitive dissonance. Since, his initial concept has been developed well beyond the original idea introduced by him. Harmon-Jones (2019) provides the directions in which this idea has developed since then.

  9. 9.

    Leippe and Eisenstadt (1994) observed how white American students being compelled to write articles favoring a policy favoring black minorities favorably impacted their attitudes on the policy; Cooper and Feldman (2019) studied how dissonance was induced for older individuals through induced compliance paradigm and observed that dissonance could be created even at older ages.

  10. 10.

    Harmon-Jones and Mills (2019) at p. 8—”When there is no choice about engaging in the behavior, dissonance is minimal, because there is sufficient justification for the behavior.”

  11. 11.

    Supra Chap. 3, 74–75.

  12. 12.

    This is, of course, presuming that if given a choice they would rather not be paying dowry.

  13. 13.

    In this case, it is also important to note that even if we expect that we could bribe the traffic police and get away from the legally imposed fine, there is still a cost associated with non-compliance, i.e., the cost of the bribe. The least-cost behavioral response is still to wear the seatbelt. Therefore, compliance through acquiescence is more likely to create and maintain initial compliance than in other more complex social situations like dowry and FGM practice (in which there are greater benefits from non-compliance like securing the future of one’s daughter).

Bibliography

Journal Articles and Book Chapters

  • Akers, R. L., & Jennings, W. G. (2019). The social learning theory of crime and deviance. In M. Krohn, N. Hendrix, G. Hall, & A. Lizotte (Eds.), The Handbook on Crime and Deviance (2nd ed., pp. 103-116).

    Google Scholar 

  • Akers, R. L., & Jensen, G. (2008). The empirical status of social learning theory of crime and deviance: The past, present, and future. In F. T. Cullen, J. P. Wright, & K. R. Blevins (Eds.), Taking stock: The status of criminological theory (pp. 37-76).

    Google Scholar 

  • Alfano, M. (2017). Daughters, Dowries, Deliveries: The Effect of Marital Payments on Fertility Choices in India. Journal of Development Economics, 125, 89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, C., Mendes, S., & Tverdova, Y. (2004). Endogenous economic voting: Evidence from the 1997 British Election. Electoral Studies, 23(4), 683–708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, M. (1963). Effect of the severity of threat on the devaluation of forbidden behavior. Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 66(6), 584-588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barreto, N., & Hogg, M. (2017). Evaluation of and support for group prototypical leaders: A meta-analysis of twenty years of empirical research. Social Influence, 12, 41–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beasley, R., & Joslyn, M. (2001). Cognitive dissonance and post-decision attitude change in six presidential elections. Political Psychology, 22(3), 521–540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bicchieri, C., & McNally, P. (2018). Shrieking sirens: Schemata, scripts, and social norms. How change occurs. Social Philosophy & Policy, 35(1), 23-53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bilgrei, O. R., Buvik, K., et al. (2021). Cannabis, youth, and social identity: The evolving meaning of cannabis use in adolescence. Journal of Youth Studies, 25, 1199-214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. (2017). Intergroup discrimination: Ingroup love, or outgroup hate? In C. Sibley & F. Barlow (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice (pp. 90-110).

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, P., & Cerven, C. (2019). Identity accumulation, verification and well-being. In J. Stets & R. Serpe (Eds.), Identities in Everyday Life (pp. 17-33).

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, A., Fehr, E., et al. (2014). Business culture and dishonesty in the banking industry. Nature, 516, 86–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, A., Maréchal, M. A., & Noll, T. (2015). Bad boys: How criminal identity salience affects rule violation. Review of Economic Studies, 82(4), 1289–1308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J., & Feldman, L. (2019). Does cognitive dissonance occur in older age? A study of induced compliance in a healthy elderly population. Psychology and Aging, 34(5), 709–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, E. (2020). The influence of identity salience on framing effectiveness: An experiment. Political Psychology, 41(6), 1133-1150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dong, B., Dulleck, U., & Torgler, B. (2012). Conditional corruption. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 609-627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dowsett, E., Semmler, C., et al. (2018). Neutralizing the meat paradox: Cognitive dissonance, gender, and eating animals. Appetite, 123, 280-288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, C. (2008). Learning the law. Journal of Institutional Economics, 4(2), 275–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epitropaki, O., Kark, R., et al. (2017). Leadership and followership identity processes: A multilevel review. The Leadership Quarterly, 28, 104–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frey, B., & Torgler, B. (2007). Tax morale and conditional cooperation. Journal of Comparative Economics, 35, 136-159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glaeser, E., Sacerdote, B., & Scheinkman, J. (1996). Crime and social interactions. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 111(2), 507-548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grusec, J. (2011). Socialization processes in the family: Social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 243-269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2007). Cognitive Dissonance Theory After 50 years of Development. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 38, 7-16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Amodio, D. M., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2009). Action-Based Model of Dissonance: A Review, Integration, and Expansion of Conceptions of Cognitive Conflict. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 119-166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., Gerjikodov, T., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2008). The effect of induced compliance on relative left frontal cortical activity: a test of the action-based model of dissonance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(1), 35-45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (Eds.). (2019). Cognitive dissonance: Re-examining a pivotal theory in psychology. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayek, F. (1948). The Use of Knowledge in Society. In Individualism and Economic Order (pp. 77–91).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hogg, M. (2003). Social Identity. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of Self and Identity (pp. 462–479).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, R. A., Gunningham, N., & Thornton, D. (2011). Fear, Duty, and Regulatory Compliance: Lessons from Three Research Projects. In V. Nielsen & C. Parker (Eds.), Explaining Compliance (pp. 37-58).

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahane, L. (2021). Politicizing the Mask: Political, Economic and Demographic Factors Affecting Mask Wearing Behavior in the USA. East Economic Journal, 47(2), 163-183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamel, I., Wahab, S., et al. (2021). Public Attitude as a Determinant of Petty Corruption in Egypt: A Survey Study. Journal of Humanities & Applied Sciences, 4(5), 481-96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelman, H. (2006). Interests, Relationships, Identities: Three Central Issues for Individuals and Groups in Negotiating Their Social Environment. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 1-26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kimemia, D. (2018). Multinational corporations as supplier of corruption in Africa. Africa Insight, 49(2), 24-40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klar, S. (2013). The Influence of Competing Identity Primes on Political Preferences. Journal of Politics, 75, 1108–1124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobi, N. C. (2018). A social psychological view on social norms of corruption. In I. Kubbe & A. Engelbert (Eds.), Corruption and Norms: Why Informal Rules Matter (pp. 31-52).

    Google Scholar 

  • Köbis, N. C., van Prooijen, J-W., et al. (2015). “Who Doesn’t?”—The Impact of Descriptive Norms on Corruption. PLOS ONE, 10(6). [Link to the article: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0131830]

  • Leippe, M., & Eisenstadt, D. (1994). Generalization of dissonance reduction: Decreasing prejudice through induced compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 395–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, G. (1996). Ending Foot-binding and Infibulation: A Convention Account. American Sociological Review, 61(6), 999-1017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martiny, S., & Rubin, M. (2016). Towards a clearer understanding of social identity theory’s self-esteem hypothesis. In S. McKeown, R. Haji, & N. Ferguson (Eds.), Understanding Peace and Conflict Through Social Identity Theory: Contemporary Global Perspectives (pp. 19-32).

    Google Scholar 

  • McAdams, R. (1997). The Origin, Development and Regulations of Norms. Michigan Law Review, 96(2), 338-433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCaffrey, D., & Keys, J. (2000). Competitive Framing Processes in the Abortion Debate: Polarization-Vilification, Frame Saving, and Frame Debunking. Sociological Quarterly, 41(1), 41-61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McKimmie, B., Hogg, M., et al. (2003). I'm a hypocrite, but so is everyone else: Group support and the reduction of cognitive dissonance. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7(3), 214–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mohan, D. (2010). Seat Belt Law Road Traffic Injuries in Delhi, India. Journal of Eastern Asia Society for Transportation Studies, 8, 2140-46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mullainathan, S., & Washington, E. (2009). Sticking with Your Vote: Cognitive Dissonance and Political Attitudes. American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 1(1), 86–111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakayachi, K., Ozaki, T., et al. (2020). Why Do Japanese People Use Masks Against COVID-19, Even Though Masks Are Unlikely to Offer Protection From Infection? Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 1918-1920.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Netter, B. (2006). Avoiding the Shameful Backlash: Social Repercussions for the Increased Use of Alternative Sanctions. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 96(1), 187-216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powdthavee, N., Wong, E., et al. (2021). When face masks signal social identity: Explaining the deep face-mask divide during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLOS ONE, 16(6), e0253195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pressman, L. (2017). How Evolution was used to Support Scientific Racism. The Trinity Papers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Regan, D., & Kilduff, M. (1988). Optimism about Elections: Dissonance Reduction at the Ballot Box. Political Psychology, 9(1), 101-107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reimer, N., Schmid, K., et al. (2020). Self-categorization and social identification: Making sense of us and them. In D. Chadee (Ed.), Theories in Social Psychology (pp. 1-30).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rotherberger, M. (2020). Meat-related cognitive dissonance: A conceptual framework for understanding how meat eaters reduce negative arousal from eating animals. Appetite, 146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roy, S. (2022a). Theory of Social Proof and Legal Compliance: A Socio-Cognitive Explanation for Regulatory (Non) Compliance. German Law Journal, 22(2), 238-255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roy, S. (2022b). When law defeats its purpose: How anti-dowry legislations (fail to) address the norms that motivate dowry payments in India. Journal of Indian Law and Society, 13(1), 51-66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutledge, D. (1995). Social Darwinism, scientific racism, and the metaphysics of race. Journal of Negro Education, 64(3), 243–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, D. (2003). Shame, stigma, and crime: Evaluating the efficacy of shaming sanctions in criminal law. Harvard Law Review, 116(7), 2186-2207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steele, C., Spencer, S., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 379-440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, J., Wiegand, A., et al. (1997). When exemplification fails: Hypocrisy and the motive for self-integrity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(1), 54–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swee Jin-Ong, A., Frewer, L., & Chan, M-Y. (2017). Cognitive dissonance in food and nutrition: A review. Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition, 57(11), 2330–2342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tay, R. (2005). General and Specific Deterrent Effects of Traffic Enforcement: Do We Have to Catch Offenders to Reduce Crashes? Journal of Transport Economics & Policy, 39(2), 209-223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornton, D., Kagan, R. A., & Gunningham, N. (2005). General Deterrence and Corporate Environmental Behavior. Law & Policy, 27, 262-288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wojcieszak, M., & Garett, K. (2018). Social Identity, Selective Exposure, and Affective Polarization: How Priming National Identity Shapes Attitudes Toward Immigrants Via News Selection. Human Communication Research, 44(3), 247–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zakeri, M. (2016). Multinational Corporations and Corruption in Africa. Journal of Management and Social Sciences, 5(2), 81–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, B., (2017). MNCs, Rents, and Corruption: Evidence from China. American Journal of Political Science, 61(1), 84–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Books

  • Akers, R. L. (2017). Social Learning and Social Structure: A General Theory of Crime and Deviance. New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Friedman, L. (2016). Impact: How Law Affects Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Harmon-Jones, E. (2019). Cognitive dissonance: Re-examining a pivotal theory in psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Harrington, A. (2019). Mind Fixers: Psychiatry’s Troubled Search for the Biology of Mental Illness. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelling, G., & Coles, C. (1996). Fixing Broken Windows: Restoring Order and Reducing Crime in Our Communities. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuran, T. (1997). Private Truth, Public Lies: The Social Consequences of Preference Falsification. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morvon, C., & O’Connor, A. (2017). An Analysis of Leon Festinger’s A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. London: Macat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, E. (1947). Principles of Criminology. Lanham: General Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trinker, R., & Tyler, T. (2018). Why Children Follow Rules: Legal Socialization and the Development of Legitimacy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Working Paper

Reports

Internet Articles and Websites

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shubhangi Roy .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2024 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Roy, S. (2024). When Law Changes Attitudes Within the Integrated Approach. In: When Do People Obey Laws?. International Law and Economics. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-53055-5_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics