Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Research in Mathematics Education ((RME))

  • 207 Accesses

Abstract

Piaget defined logico-mathematical operations as reversible and composable mental actions. Because of this, formal mathematical structures, such as algebraic groups, played important roles in Piaget’s genetic epistemology. Group-like structures describe ways that mental actions can be organized and composed with one another. For example, mental actions of partitioning can be composed with one another to partition a partitioned whole, and such mental actions can be reversed by iterating one of the parts to reproduce the whole. This chapter details the ways that Piaget relied on groups and group-like structures to build models of mathematical development. Whereas research in mathematics education often refers to schemes as structures for organizing mental actions, it rarely mentions group-like structures. The chapter draws on the example of the splitting loope/group to illustrate how formal algebraic structures—as researcher constructs—can be useful in modeling children’s mathematical development. It also includes related exposition on Piaget’s INRC group, Klein’s Erlangen program, Noether’s algebraic invariants, and the mother structures identified by the Bourbaki.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 149.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 199.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1.

    Historians credit Arthur Cayley for providing the first definition of an abstract group, in 1954. See Kleiner (1986) for a thorough and engaging review of its history, including contributions from Viète, Descartes, and (especially) Galois.

  2. 2.

    Here, Piaget is referring to Bertrand Russell’s circular definition of number, which Piaget (1971) critiques in Genetic Epistemology (see pp. 36–37).

  3. 3.

    “We are not given mathematical objects in isolation but rather in structures. That 13 is a prime number is not determined by some internal property of 13 but rather by its place in the structure of the natural numbers” (Resnick, 1981, p. 529).

  4. 4.

    Note that all finite groups satisfy the Latin squares property because they are associative, which is an even stronger condition. We can use associativity and the other properties of a group to prove that every element in a finite group appears exactly once in each row and column of the Cayley table.

  5. 5.

    Among books that have been translated to English, Psychology of Intelligence (2001/1947) and Mathematical Psychology and Epistemology (1966) stand out, so those are used as chief references on groupings.

  6. 6.

    Even within the same text, Piaget (1972a) sometimes referred to this relationship as idempotent (p. 90) and in other paces referred it to tautology (p. 97). It is not clear why.

  7. 7.

    In the case of multiplicative relations, Beth and Piaget (1966) characterized this dimension as bi-univocal (one to one, like a multiplication table) verses co-univocal (many to one, like tree branching).

  8. 8.

    Piaget (2001/1947) described the groupings of concrete operations as having a qualitative (not yet quantitative) character.

  9. 9.

    Piaget and Inhelder (1967/1948) acknowledge the influence of both Kant and Poincaré, at the start of their book, The Child’s Conception of Space.

  10. 10.

    Note that even algebraic manipulation relies on algebraic invariance: “manipulating a system of equations but maintaining its solution set” (Harel, 2008, p. 15).

  11. 11.

    Piaget notes as much in a separate account of the mother structures or “matrix structures” (Beth & Piaget, 1966, p. 164).

References

  • Ascher, E. (1984). The case of Piaget’s group INRC. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 28, 282–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beth & Piaget (1966). Mathematical epistemology and psychology (Trans. W. Mays). Gordon and Breach.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J., & Smith, E. (1995). Splitting, covariation, and their role in the development of exponential functions. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26(1), 66–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davydov, V. V. (1992). The psychological analysis of multiplication procedures. Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 14(1), 3–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, P., & Norton, A. (2022). Identifying mental actions necessary for abstracting the logic of conditionals. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 66, 100954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Easley, J. A. (1964). Comments on the INRC group. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 2, 233–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, B. (2011). Inversion in mathematical thinking and learning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 79, 429–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackenberg, A. J. (2007). Units coordination and the construction of improper fractions: A revision of the splitting hypothesis. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 26, 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackenberg, A. J. (2010). Students’ reasoning with reversible multiplicative relationships. Cognition and Instruction, 28(4), 383–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harel, G. (2008). What is mathematics? A pedagogical answer to a philosophical question. In R. B. Gold & R. Simons (Eds.), Current issues in the philosophy of mathematics from the perspective of mathematicians. Mathematical American Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1969). The early growth of logic in the child (E. A. Lunzer & E. Papert, Trans.). Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1998). Critique of pure reason (Trans. P. Guyer & A. Wood). Cambridge University Press. (Original work published in 1781).

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, F. (1893). A comparative review of recent researches in geometry. (Trans. M. W. Haskell). Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society, 2(10), 215–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiner, I. (1986). The evolution of group theory: A brief survey. Mathematics Magazine, 59(4), 195–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mancosu, P. (1998). From Brouwer to Hilbert: The debate on the foundations of mathematics in the 1920s. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council. (2009). Mathematics learning in early childhood: Paths toward excellence and equity. National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norton, A. (2016). (Ir)reversibility in mathematics. Proceedings of the thirty-eighth annual meeting of the north American chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of mathematics education. University of Arizona.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norton, A., & Wilkins, J. L. (2010). Students’ partitive reasoning. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 29(4), 181–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norton, A., & Wilkins, J. L. M. (2012). The splitting group. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 43(5), 557–583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olive, J., & Steffe, L. P. (2002). The construction of an iterative fractional scheme: The case of joe. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20, 413–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pascal, B. (1966). Pensées (A. J. Krailsheimer, Trans.). Penguin (Original work published in 1670).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1969). The child’s conception of time (A. J. Pomerans, Trans.). Ballantine (Original work published 1946).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1970). Structuralism (C. Maschler, Trans.). Basic Books (Original work published 1968).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1971). Genetic epistemology (E. Duckworth, Trans.). Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1972a). Essai de Logique Opératoire (2nd ed.). Dunod. (Original work published in 1949).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1972b). Principles of genetic epistemology (W. Mays, Trans.). Routledge & Kegan Paul (Original work published in 1970).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1973). The child and reality: Problems of genetic psychology. (A. Rosin, Trans.). Grossman Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development (T. Brown & K. J. Thampy, Trans.). University of Chicago Press (Original work published in 1975).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (2001). The psychology of intelligence. London: Routledge (M. Piercy & D. E. Beryne, Trans.). Routledge Classics (Original work published 1947).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child’s conception of space (F.J. Langdon & J.L. Lunzer, Trans.). Norton (Original work published in 1948).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Szeminska, A. (1952). The child’s conception of number. Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poincaré, H. (1952). Science and hypothesis. Dover (Original work published in 1905).

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnik, M. D. (1981). Mathematics as a science of patterns: Ontology and reference. Nous, 529–550.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowe, D. E., & Koreuber, M. (2020). Proving it her way: Emmy Noether, a life in mathematics. Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Richards, D. D. (1979). Development of time, speed, and distance concepts. Developmental Psychology, 15(3), 288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silver, E. A., Leung, S. S., & Cai, J. (1995). Generating multiple solutions for a problem: A comparison of the responses of US and Japanese students. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 28(1), 35–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, M. A., Kara, M., Placa, N., & Sandir, H. (2016). Categorizing and promoting reversibility of mathematical concepts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 93(2), 137–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steen, L. A. (1988). The science of patterns. Science, 240(4852), 611–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P. (1992). Schemes of action and operation involving composite units. Learning and Individual Differences, 4(3), 259–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/1041-6080(92)90005-Y

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P. (2002). A new hypothesis concerning children’s fractional knowledge. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20(3), 267–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steffe, L. P., & D’Ambrosio, B. S. (1995). Toward a working model of constructivist teaching: A reaction to Simon. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26, 146–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tall, D., Thomas, M., Davis, G., Gray, E., & Simpson, A. (2000). What is the object of the encapsulation of a process? The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 18(2), 223–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzur, R. (1999). An integrated study of children’s construction of improper fractions and the teacher’s role in promoting that learning. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 30(4), 390–416.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, C. L. (2012). Additive relationships and signed quantities. Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia).

    Google Scholar 

  • von Glasersfeld, E. (1995). Radical constructivism: A way of knowing and learning. Routledge Falmer.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Glasersfeld, E., & Steffe, L. P. (1991). Conceptual models in educational research and practice. The Journal of Educational Thought, 25(2), 91–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wessman-Enzinger, N. M. (2019). Integers as directed quantities. In A. Norton & M. Alibali (Eds.), Constructing number (pp. 279–305). Cham.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wilkins, J. L. M., & Norton, A. (2011). The splitting loope. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 42(4), 386–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, E. (1973). The concept of grouping in Jean Piaget’s psychology—Formalization and applications. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 5, 125–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anderson Norton .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2024 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Norton, A. (2024). Groups and Group-Like Structures. In: Dawkins, P.C., Hackenberg, A.J., Norton, A. (eds) Piaget’s Genetic Epistemology for Mathematics Education Research. Research in Mathematics Education. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-47386-9_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-47386-9_7

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-031-47385-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-031-47386-9

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics