Skip to main content

A Post-psychology of Motivation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Rearticulating Motives

Part of the book series: Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences ((THHSS))

  • 83 Accesses

Abstract

In this chapter you will be offered a ‘post-psychology’ that traces concepts of motivation and motives through various theories relevant to my project. The most important problem, around which this ‘immanent critique’ is centered, is that of scientific objectivity: How to make of subjectivity a scientific object? Or, how to move beyond this contradictory ambition? As already mentioned, the concept of ‘boundary objectivity’ around objects such as ‘energy’ are suggested as central. We shall discuss prevalent strands of positivist psychology (behavior design, cognitive theories, self-determination theory), and then off-mainstream traditions. Of the latter, I focus mostly on the Vygotskian (socio-cultural-historical) tradition, including its version of critical psychology, but I also venture to find parallels to (critical) psychoanalysis. When off-mainstream traditions seek a scientific objectivity for their concepts of motives, the idea of (biological and social) ‘function’ becomes important, as it configures the necessity of needs, defines pathologies and cures, and translates those into activities. This has spurred attempts to overcome such functionalism, and the dualisms it entails; attempts, which then run into troubles with how to think of objectivity. That contradiction is the germ cell from which the position of this book has emerged.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The EARLI special interest group 8 on motivation and emotion conference “Design for Motivation and Emotion”, Aarhus 2018.

  2. 2.

    Klaus Holzkamp”s (2011) term for the kind of psychology that proceeds by correlating two variables to find what causes behavior.

  3. 3.

    In Chap. 3, we shall dive more deeply into this issue, as we consider the relations between signs as things and as objects.

  4. 4.

    Another example is given in Solgaard and Nissen (2021): The use of personality tests in practices of hiring. Here, participants are vaguely aware of the limited validity of test scores, but move back and forth between referring to them as given facts, negotiating their meaning, and reflecting their implications.

  5. 5.

    This sets in motion a continuous process of verification that gradually undermines veridity because the formal transparency of procedures masks rather than reconciles or overcomes contradictions of meaning. Hans Rosling’s popular and well-intended “factfulness” (Rosling et al., 2018) is to pee in your pants to stay warm in the deepening winter. The rise and fall of the Danish anti-environmentalist Bjørn Lomborg, who was endorsed and financed by liberalist and neoconservative Danish governments to promote “climate skepticism” with pure evidence, is an illustrative case. Jamison (2004) laments Lomborg’s contempt for real science and calls for new procedures to secure factfulness; yet his reconstruction of the political and institutional backgrounds of Lomborg’s rise to fame rather suggests a wider contextualization of such “facts” in ideological struggles. Lomborg’s trick was precisely to substitute a formalized procedure of evidence for the consistencies of climate science that were and are intimately related to the politics and the publics of climate activism, both before and after Lomborg. While Lomborg’s easy rise to power is an occasion for grave concerns, his fall is encouraging; yet, what has been generally learnt from it?

  6. 6.

    This is true of plants as well, but the scarce resource in plant “economy”, is nitrogen, rather than energy.

  7. 7.

    The pseudo-mathematical representation of theories is a more general trope in psychology (Lewin, Valsiner, and many others), as well as in other social sciences. Rather than a false dressing designed to be allowed access to “science,” this form should be analyzed as a way to invoke the hope of a science that can cumulate a body of knowledge through formalizing stipulated theorems.

  8. 8.

    Even with the theorizing of life implied in sexuality, in self-preservation and the death drive, psychoanalysis never (to my knowledge) thought of basing the idea of a libidinal economy on the finitude of life—everyday life as well as life course – as it would be developed in existentialism. Perhaps this was a result of its clinical focus, life viewed as “zoë” rather than as “bios.”

  9. 9.

    Later, “motivation,” as an already well-established object, does appear there, as well as in more recent standardizations such as health promotion and sports.

  10. 10.

    Valverde (1998) recounts how some early 20th century alcoholism programs—before “motivation” —would emphasize displays of will, taking inspiration from the gymnastics of the time (which, in turn, reappeared later as “fitness” and “body-building”).

  11. 11.

    Kahneman, who earned a Nobel prize in economics, makes a note of how apt that phrase is: Paying attention. “You dispose of a limited budget of attention” (ibid., 23).

  12. 12.

    Anyone writing in English will be familiar with the warning built into the algorithms of the Word program: “Passive voice. Consider revising.” Apparently, passive voice spends the reader’s limited System 2 budget. And I don’t even have to know how or why—just trust and go on writing the canonical language.

  13. 13.

    Probably first of all the interactive or customizable nature of “hyper-text” and the coexistence of multiple platforms on the same hardware device; as we shall see in Chap. 4, this logic of a commodity or brand defining a fixed frame with customized items (all standardized) is recognizable also in domains such as counselling.

  14. 14.

    The “Copenhagen Consensus Center” initiated by Lomborg is a case in point—see (June, 2023) https://www.copenhagenconsensus.com/.

  15. 15.

    The idea that causal explanations in psychology masked the affirmation of a generally accepted rationality was already proposed by Holzkamp (1993), who, in turn, based partly on Smedslund (2012 [1988]). For my purposes, I propose to free this hypothesis from its place in an argument for a new realist psychology of reasons (cf. Sect. 2.12). Instead, my attempt is to use it in a post-psychology that seeks to understand the performance of causal psychologies addressing reasons.

  16. 16.

    Note that the concept of “common sense” is not, as I intend to use it, pejorative. Rather, I think of it as an object of study and of a reflective self-overcoming – mindful that the task of a theorist is to suggest new ways of thinking, thus to move beyond common sense, in ways that are always precarious (cf. Nissen, 2012b, Chap. 7).

  17. 17.

    In Bowker and Star (1999), these are dubbed NSS statements: No shit, Sherlock? – too obvious to be taken seriously as instructions to nursing practice. Yet, mainstream psychology is virtually made of these.

  18. 18.

    Thus, as “Nature” as defined in Chap. 1. This may, but need not, be “anchored” in an “ultimate” reference to an organic substance – most typically, the brain.

  19. 19.

    This can even be argued within their own logic: The priming strengthened the association “cocaine-research” —which was probably already established with the remuneration given to participants—except, in this genre, participants always try to do what they are told, or they are deleted from reported findings.

  20. 20.

    For a realist critique of this methodology, see Chirkov and Anderson (2018).

  21. 21.

    Imagine how funny your date would find it when you checked the “funny meter”; or, imagine how you would interpret him/her replying that my joke is 7 on a scale from 1 to 10, after consulting his/her “funny meter”. Perhaps just as another joke? Or, maybe stuff for another episode of the dystopian sci-fi TV show “Black Mirror”?

  22. 22.

    Thus, even though professionals in social work and in schools often experience the wave of “evidence-based practice” as imported from the medical field, it actually originated in educational psychology.

  23. 23.

    See Motzkau (2007) for a more contextualized approach to suggestion.

  24. 24.

    SCHAT is an inclusive way of naming the tradition, without separating the “cultural-historical” from the “sociocultural”. The former has been more European, and continued more from the further development of Vygotsky’s theories by A.N. Leontiev and his colleagues in and around Moscow, while the latter has been more Anglo-Saxon and took off more directly from translations of Vygotsky. Part of this history is about translations. Another part is about how “CHAT” has been widely institutionalized with reference to the works of Engeström and colleagues as representing a “3rd generation” of a “dynasty” after Vygotsky and Leontiev. The more inclusive name fits the profile of the association and its conferences (see https://www.iscar.org/), and it is more relevant here—even though, as we shall see, it was Leontiev who developed a proper theory of motives.

  25. 25.

    See (June 2023) https://apps.who.int/gb/bd/PDF/bd47/EN/constitution-en.pdf

  26. 26.

    As the framing of a psychology (sui generis, independent of other social theories), this functionalism is rarely articulated explicitly as such, even if it could be, obviously, with reference to such social theories as those of Malinowsky or Parsons. The exception to this rule is perhaps certain Freudo-Marxist socialization theories that explicitly took the “reproduction of capital” as their overall point of reference (e.g., Lorenzer, 1973).

  27. 27.

    In the specific article quoted here, Hedegaard is ambivalent about the term ‘need’ itself. She recapitulates and confirms Leontiev’s above-quoted reversal of the cycles of actions and needs (p. 16), but she does not explicitly take up ‘functional-objective’ or ‘higher cultural’ needs. In her ontological Table 1.1. on ‘Planes of analysis,’ ‘needs’ figure at the top as ‘social needs’ and again at the bottom as the ‘primary needs’ at the level of ‘human biology’ (p. 19). She rearticulates Bozhovich’ use of the term ‘need’ as a ‘motive factor’ (p. 22, note 2), that is, as conceptualizing the structuring of motives, claiming only implicitly that the functional objectivity of the social needs of ‘activity settings’ determines such ‘motive factors’ as ‘needs.’

  28. 28.

    In fact, materialism is quite often idealistic, when ‘matter’ is conceived as formless, that is, the form given by the conceptualization itself is ignored and projected onto ‘matter’ itself.

  29. 29.

    In earlier writings, I have used the term “German-Scandinavian Critical Psychology,” but I realize that important contributions have been Finnish

  30. 30.

    It should be noted that Holzkamp and Osterkamp were a couple, who worked together and influenced each other’s works to a large degree—and that Osterkamp devoted most of her efforts after Holzkamp’s death in 1995 to representing “his” legacy.

  31. 31.

    It is instructive to compare with the phenomenological sociology of Schütz, Garfinkel and others, despite their more direct acknowledgement of the Husserlian legacy. Here, phenomenology is smoothly rearticulated on the basis of sociology’s foundational assumption of the rational agent. Since Durkheim and Tönnies, the bracketing of the possible irrationality of less-than-social individuals such as children, natives, mental patients, etc. was part of constituting a “sui generis” social science. Even Goffman (1961) had to delimit his study of Asylums from any claims about the objects of psychiatrists’ expertise (cf. Nissen, 2012b, Ch. 3).

  32. 32.

    Incidentally, and interestingly, Osterkamp’s careful and respectful rearticulation of Freud’s work is directly opposite to Holzkamp’s hasty appropriation of phenomenology.

  33. 33.

    The general narrative seems to be that Alexandre Kojève’s lectures on Hegel’s Phenomenology (Kojève, 1980), attended by Lacan, Sartre, and others, were a founding moment. I am much indebted to Judith Butler’s thorough reconstruction of the French reception of Hegel and of the concept of desire, although a full appreciation of this work and its object is beyond my scope.

  34. 34.

    Although Holzkamp participated in an attempt at rearticulating the clinic already in Kappeler et al. (1977), that GNCP “practice research” remained undeveloped, and even when it took off as a program in the 1990s (cf. Markard & Holzkamp, 1989; Fahl & Markard, 1999; and Nissen, 2000), it only half-heartedly problematized the structure and discourse of the clinic.

  35. 35.

    Butler (1997) discusses the way this problem is related to embodiment and to the possibility of resistance. In this respect, her project can be seen as similar to that of GNCP.

  36. 36.

    It is also important in aesthetics – as we shall see (Chap. 5): Does Rancièrian dissensus refute the singularity of the artwork, or is aesthetic creation precisely an individuation that temporarily unites contradictory regimes of sense in a paradoxical consistence?

  37. 37.

    In and of itself, this idea could simply, as in Illouz’ equally convincing reading, express an emerging capitalist consumer culture, rather than a universal aspect of human life (Illouz, 2019).

  38. 38.

    “Freudian Repression” is a part of this book by Stiegler, and also the title of a book by Michael Billig printed in the same year (1999). Interestingly, while Stiegler highlights technology but ignores activity, the opposite is the case with Billig, who convincingly rearticulates Freud’s analyses as “repressed” expressions of forms of social interaction and discourse as conversation. Thus, they each highlight an aspect of the social practice that is missing in Freud.

  39. 39.

    An example of this are the attempts to characterize the problems that arise from the recent sudden rise in the use of “screens”, where such technologies are seen as damaging since disturbing healthy, “natural” relations between “mother and child” as well as the natural sequence of ontogenesis, which, it seems, must recapitulate that of history (e.g. talk → text → screens) (cf. Stiegler, 2010a; Bossière, 2021).

References

  • Adorno, T. W., & Horkheimer, M. (1979). Dialectic of enlightenment. Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health – How people manage stress and stay well. Jossey-Bass Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41(3), 586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1972b). Steps to an ecology of mind. Ballantine Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. (1966). The social construction of reality. A treatise in the sociology of knowledge. Doubleday Anchor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Billig, M. (1991). Ideology and opinions. Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloch, E. (1995). The principle of hope. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bossière, M.-C. (2021). Le Bébé au Temps du Numérique. L’humanité au risque des disrupteurs relationnels. Hermann Éditeurs.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bourgois, P. (2003). In search of respect. Selling crack in El Barrio. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowker, G., & Star, S. L. (1999). Sorting things out. Classification and its consequences. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brecht, B. (1971). Über Politik auf dem Theater. Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brecht, B. (1982). Über experimentelles Theater. Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brecht, B., & Unseld, S. (1993). Schriften zum Theater: über eine nicht-aristotelische Dramatik. Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinkmann, S. (2006). Psychology as a moral science. Psykologisk Institut, Aarhus Universitet.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butler, J. (1997). The psychic life of power. Theories in subjection. Stanford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Butler, J. (2012). Subjects of desire: Hegelian reflections in twentieth-century France. Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Canguilhem, G. (1991). The normal and the pathological. Zone Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chertok, L., & Stengers, I. (1992). A critique of psychoanalytic reason: Hypnosis as a scientific problem from Lavoisier to Lacan. Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chimirri, N. A. (2019). Specifying the ethics of teleogenetic collaboration for research with children and other vital forces: A critical inquiry into dialectical praxis psychology via posthumanist theorizing. Human Arenas, 2(4), 451–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chirkov, V., & Anderson, J. (2018). Statistical positivism versus critical scientific realism. A comparison of two paradigms for motivation research: Part 1. A philosophical and empirical analysis of statistical positivism. Theory & Psychology, 28(6), 712–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., & Consortium, D. L. (2006). The fifth dimension: An after-school program built on diversity. Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daggett, C. N. (2019). The birth of energy: Fossil fuels, thermodynamics and the politics of work. Duke University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1994). Constructing the subject: Historical origins of psychological research. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind. How psycholog found its language. Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Davydov, V. V. (1973). Arten der Verallgemeinerung im Unterricht. Volk & Wissen Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davydov, V. V., & Markova, A. K. (1983). A concept of educational activity for schoolchildren. Soviet Psychology, 21(1), 50–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (1994). Specters of Marx: The state of the debt, the work of mourning & the new international. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (2016). Of grammatology. Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dreier, O. (2008). Psychotherapy in everyday life. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, A. (2011). Building common knowledge at the boundaries between professional practices: Relational agency and relational expertise in systems of distributed expertise. International Journal of Educational Research, 50(1), 33–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fahl, R., & Markard, M. (1999). The project “analysis of psychological practice” or: An attempt at connecting psychology critique and practice research. Outlines – Critical Social Studies, 1(1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleer, M., et al. (2017). Perezhivanie, emotions and subjectivity. Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1967). Madness and civilization. A history of insanity in the age of reason. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1973a). The birth of the clinic. Tavistock Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1973b). The order of things. Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1985). The history of sexuality I-III. Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1997a). Discipline and punish. The birth of the prison. Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1937). Analysis terminable and interminable, Standard Edition (Vol. 23). Hogarth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giddens, A. (1984). The constitution of society. Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1961). Asylums. Essays on the social situation of mental patients and other inmates. Penguin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez Rey, F. L. (2015). Human motivation in question: Discussing emotions, motives, and subjectivity from a cultural‐historical standpoint. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 45(4), 419–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • González Rey, F. (2017). The topic of subjectivity in psychology: Contradictions, paths and new alternatives. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 47(4), 502–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • González Rey, F. (2019a). Subjectivity within cultural-historical approach. Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • González Rey, F. L. (2019b). Subjectivity and discourse: Complementary topics for a critical psychology. Culture & Psychology, 25(2), 178–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graumann, C. F. (1985). Phänomenologische Analytik und experimentelle Methodik in der Psychologie-das Problem der Vermittlung. In K. H. Braun (Ed.), Subjektivität als Problem psychologischer Methodik (pp. 38–59). Pahl-Rugenstein.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. (1970). Technology and science as «ideology». In Toward a rational society (pp. 81–122). Beacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, D. (2000). Spaces of hope. Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haug, F. (1977). Erziehung und gesellschaftliche Produktion. Kritik des Rollenspiels. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haug, W. F. (1983). Hält das Ideologische Subjekt Einzug in die Kritische Psychologie? Forum Kritische Psychologie, 11, 24–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haug, F. (1999). Female sexualisation. A collective work of memory. Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haug, F. (2002). Vorlesungen zur Einfhrung in die Erinnerungsarbeit. The Duke lectures. Argument.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haug, F. (2003). LernverHältnisse. Selbstbewegungen und Selbstblockierungen. Argument.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haug, F., & Blankenburg, U. (1980). Frauenformen. Argument.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. (2011). The dynamic aspects in children’s learning and development. In M. Hedegaard, A. Edwards, & M. Fleer (Eds.), Motives in children’s development. Cultural-historical approaches (pp. 9–27). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hedegaard, M. H. P., & Engeström, Y. (Eds.). (1984). Learning and teaching on a scientific basis: Methodological and epistemological aspects of the activity theory of learning and teaching. Aarhus Universitet Psykologisk Institut.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1963). Sein und Zeit (1927). De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1977). The question concerning technology and othe ressays. Garland Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hertwig, R., & Gigerenzer, G. (1999). The ‘conjunction fallacy’ revisited: How intelligent inferences look like reasoning errors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 12(4), 275–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeister, A. (1998). Zur Kritik des Bildungsbegriffs aus subjektwissenschaftlicher Perspektive. Diskursanalytische Untersuchungen. Argument.

    Google Scholar 

  • Højrup, T. (2003). State, culture and life-modes. The foundations of life-mode analysis. Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (1973). Sinnliche Erkenntnis – Historischer Ursprung und gesellschaftliche Funktion der Wahrnehmung. Athenäum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (1983). Grundlegung der Psychologie. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (1984). Kritische Psychologie und phänomenologische Psychologie. Der Weg der Kritischen Psychologie zur Subjektwissenschaft. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 14(5), 55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (1993). Lernen. Subjektwissenschaftliche Grundlegung. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (2011). Turning psychology upside-down. Palgrave MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Illouz, E. (2019). The end of love: A sociology of negative relations. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamison, A. (2004). Learning from Lomborg: Or where do anti‐environmentalists come from? Science as Culture, 13(2), 173–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, U. J. (1999). Categories in activity theory: Marx’ philosophy just-in-time. In S. Chaiklin, M. Hedegaard, & U. J. Jensen (Eds.), Activity theory and social practice: Cultural-historical approaches (pp. 79–99). Aarhus University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, U. J. (2007). The struggle for clinical authority: Shifting ontologies and the politics of evidence. Biosocieties, 2(1), 101–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kappeler, M., et al. (1977). Psychologische Therapie und politisches Handeln. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kierkegaard, S. (1980). The sickness unto death. A Christian psychological exposition for upbuilding and awakening. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, M. (1924). The role of the school in the libidinal development of the child. International Journal of Psycho-Analysis, 5, 312–331.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kojève, A. (1980). Introduction to the reading of Hegel. Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kontopodis, M. (2012). Neoliberalism, pedagogy and human development: Exploring time, mediation and collectivity in contemporary schools. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Köpetz, C. E., et al. (2013). Motivation and self-regulation in addiction: A call for convergence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(1), 3–24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Leontiev, A. N. (1978). Activity, consciousness, and personality. Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontiev, A. N. (2009). Activity and consciousness. Marxists Internet Archive.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontiev, A. N. (2019). The development of mind. Selected Works of A.N. Leontyev. Marxists Internet Archive Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontjew, A. N. (1979). Tätigkeit, Bewusstsein, Persönlichkeit. DDR, Volk und Wissen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontjew, A. N. (1985). Probleme der Entwicklung des Psychischen. DDR, Volk & Wissen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leroi-Gourhan, A. (1993). Gesture and speech. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lompscher, J. (1989). Aufsteigen vom Abstrakten zum Konkreten im Unterricht: Versuche zu einer alternativen Lehrstrategie. Akademie Pädagogische Wissenschaften der DDR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longo, G., et al. (2015). In search of principles for a theory of organisms. Journal of Biosciences, 40(5), 955–968.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lorenzer, A. (1973). Zur Begründung einer materialistischen Sozialisationstheorie. Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luhmann, N. (1970). Funktion und Kausalität. In Soziologische Aufklärung 1 (pp. 9–30). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markard, M., & Holzkamp, K. (1989). Praxis-portrait. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 23, 5–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marteau, T. M., et al. (2011). Judging nudging: Can nudging improve population health? BMJ, 342, d228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martela, F., & Ryan, R. M. (2016). The benefits of benevolence: Basic psychological needs, beneficence, and the enhancement of well‐being. Journal of Personality, 84(6), 750–764.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, E. (2021). Experiments of the mind. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marvakis, A. (2019). The neoliberal framing of (critical) psychology. Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 16, 22–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, K. (1986). Outline of the critique of political economy [Grundrisse]. Laurence & Wishart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marx, K. (2018). Theses on Feuerbach. http://www.marxists.org/archive/marx/works/1845/theses/theses.htm

  • Mattingly, C. (2010). The paradox of hope: Journeys through a clinical borderland. University of California Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mbembe, A. (2020). Brutalisme. La Découverte.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Minnick, N. (1993). Teacher’s directives: The social construction of ‘literal’ meanings and ‘real world’ in classroom discourse. In S. Chaiklin & J. Lave (Eds.), Understanding practice. Perspectives on activity and context (pp. 343–376). Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Minuchin, S. (2018). Families and family therapy. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, G. (2018). The pharmacology of addiction. Parrhesia, 29, 190–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mørck, L. L. (2000). Practice research and learning resources. A joint venture with the initiative ‘Wild Learning’. Outlines, 2, 61–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mørck, L. L. (2010). Expansive learning as production of community. NSSE Yearbook, 109(1), 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Motzkau, J. F. (2007). Matters of suggestibility, memory and time: Child witnesses in court and what really happened. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (1998). Ideologies and developments in practical dealings with addiction. In B. K. C. M. M. Fried & G. Wolf (Eds.), Erkenntnis und Parteilichkeit. Kritische Psychologie als marxistische Subjektwissenschaft (Vol. 254, pp. 229–240). Argument Sonderband.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (1999). Subjects, discourse and ideology in social work. In W. E. A. Maiers (Ed.), Challenges to theoretical psychology (pp. 286–294). Captus Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2000). Practice research. Critical psychology in and through practices. Annual Review of Critical Psychology, 2, 145–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2002a). To be and not to be. The subjectivity of drug taking. Outlines, 4(2), 39–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2002b). Wildes Lernen. Nachlese als Vorbereitung. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 45, 97–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2003). Objective subjectification: The anti-method of social work. Mind, Culture & Acitivity, 10(4), 332–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2004b). Das Subjekt der Kritik. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 47, 73–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2004d). Wild objectification: Social work as object. Outlines, 6(1), 73–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2005). The subjectivity of participation. Sketch of a theory. Critical Psychology, 15, 151–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2006b). Zum Standort von Kritik in der kritischen Psychologie heute. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 50(13), 30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2008). The place of a positive critique in contemporary critical psychology. Critical Social Studies – Outlines.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2009b). Objectification and prototype. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 6(1), 67–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2012b). The subjectivity of participation. Articulating social work practice with youth in Copenhagen. Palgrave MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2013). Beyond innocence and cynicism: Concrete Utopia in social work with drug users. Outlines. Critical Practice Studies, 14(2).

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2018a). Motivation erforschen durch Reartikulation. Das Argument, 326, 222–233.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2018c). Who, ‘we’? The constitution and the singular identity of the collective. Subjectivity, 11(4), 357–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2019). Remembering, rewriting, rearticulating, resituating motivation. Annual Review of Critical Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M. (2020). Critical psychology: The most recent version (soon to be replaced), illustrated by the problem of motivation. In L. B. Malich & V. Hamburg (Eds.), Psychologie und Kritik. Formen der Psychologisierung nach 1945. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, M., & Barington, K. (2016). Numbers: Manageable nothingness or user- driven standards? In J. W.-L. Bang & D. A. Winther-Lindqvist (Eds.), Nothingness (pp. 251–286). Transaction Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterkamp, U. (1975). Grundlagen der psychologischen Motivationsforschung 1. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterkamp, U. (1976). Motivationsforschung 2. Die besonderheit menschlicher Bedürfnisse – Problematik und Erkenntnisgehalt der Psychoanalyse. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, D. (2018). Experimental practice: Technoscience, alterontologies, and more-than-social movements. Duke University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, I. (1997). Psychoanalytic Culture. psychoanalytic discourse in western society. Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, I. (1999). Deconstructing psychotherapy. Sage.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Parker, I., & Pavón-Cuellár, D. (2021). Psychoanalysis and revolution. 1968 Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (2003). The psychology of intelligence. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Porter, T. M. (1996). Trust in numbers: The pursuit of objectivity in science and public life. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Przybylski, A. K., et al. (2012). The ideal self at play: The appeal of video games that let you be all you can be. Psychological Science, 23(1), 69–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raffnsøe, S. (2016a). Philosophy of the Anthropocene: The human turn. Palgrave MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raffnsøe, S. (2016b). What is critique? The critical state of critique in the age of criticism. Outlines. Critical Practice Studies, 18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reich, W. (1972). Sex-Pol. Essays 1929-1934. Vintage Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rey, F. L. G. (2007). Social and individual subjectivity from an historical cultural standpoint. Outlines. Critical Practice Studies, 9(2), 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rigby, S., & Ryan, R. M. (2011). Glued to games: How video games draw us in and hold us spellbound: How video games draw us in and hold us spellbound. ABC-CLIO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosa, H. (2013). Social acceleration: A new theory of modernity. Columbia University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, N. (1996). Inventing our selves. Psychology, power, and personhood. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, N. (1999). Powers of freedom. Reframing political thought. Cambrige University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, N. (2007). The politics of life itself. Biomedicine, power, and subjectivity in the twenty-first century. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rosling, H., et al. (2018). Factfulness: Ten reasons we’re wrong about the world–And why things are better than you think. Sceptre.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25(1), 54–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation, development, and wellness. Guilford Publications.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sackett, D. L. (2000). Evidence‐based medicine. Wiley Online Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sannino, A., & Engeström, Y. (2018). Cultural-historical activity theory: Founding insights and new challenges. Cultural-Historical Psychology, 14(3), 43–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scholer, A. A., et al. (2018). New directions in self-regulation: The role of metamotivational beliefs. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(6), 437–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schraube, E. (2013). First-person perspective and sociomaterial decentering: Studying technology from the standpoint of the subject. Subjectivity, 6(1), 12–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schüll, N. D. (2012). Addiction by design: Machine gambling in Las Vegas. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H., et al. (2014). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications. Pearson Higher Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schurig, V. (1975). Naturgeschichte des Psychischen. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schurig, V. (1976). Die Entstehung des Bewußtseins. Campus.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selvini-Palazzoli, M., et al. (1979). Paradox and counterparadox. Aronson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simondon, G. (1989). L’individuation psychique et collective à la lumière des notions de forme, information, potentiel et métastabilité. Aubier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smedslund, J. (2012). Psycho-logic. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solgaard, K. M., & Nissen, M. (2021). The potentials of a dialogical reframing of personality testing in hiring. International Review of Theoretical Psychologies, 1(2), 104–122. https://doi.org/10.7146/irtp.v1i2.128015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sørensen, E. (2013). Violent computer games in the German press. New Media & Society, 15(6), 963–981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanislavski, K. (2008). An actor’s work: A student’s diary. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Star, S. L., & Griesemer, J. R. (1989). Institutional ecology, translations’ and boundary objects: Amateurs and professionals in Berkeley’s Museum of Vertebrate Zoology, 1907-39. Social Studies of Science, 19(3), 387–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stengers, I. (1997). Power and invention. Situating science. University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenner, P. (2018a). Liminality and experience. A transdisciplinary approach to the psychosocial. Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenner, P. (2018b). Liminality and experience: A transdisciplinary approach to the psychosocial. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stetsenko, A. (2016). The transformative mind. Expanding Vygotsky’s approach to development and education. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stetsenko, A., & Arievitch, I. M. (2004). Vygotskian collaborative project of social transformation. International Journal of Critical Psychology, 14, 58–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (1998). Technics and time: The fault of Epimetheus. Stanford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2003). Passer à l’acte. Éditions Galilée.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2010a). Taking care of youth and the generations. Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2010b). Technics and time, 3: Cinematic time and the question of malaise. Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2013a). De la misère symbolique. Flammarion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2013b). What makes life worth living: On pharmacology. Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2015). States of shock: Stupidity and knowledge in the 21st century. Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2016). Dans la disruption: Comment ne pas devenir fou? Éditions Les Liens qui libèrent.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B. (2020). Nanjing lectures (2016-2019). Open Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiegler, B., et al. (2021). Bifurcate: There is no alternative. Open Humanities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, C. (1991). The ethics of authenticity. Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaler, R., & Sunstein, C. (1976). Nudge. Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorgaard, K. (2010). Evidence, patient perspective and deliberative clinical decision-making. Aarhus University. PhD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, C. W. (1994). Psychology, society, and subjectivity. An introduction to German critical psychology. Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Troels-Lund, T. F. (2008). Sundhedsbegreber i Norden i det 16 aarhundrede. BiblioBazaar, LLC..

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90(4), 293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (Ed.). (1986). The individual subject and scientific psychology. Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valverde, M. (1998). Diseases of the will. Alcohol and the Dilemmas of freedom. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Von Clausewitz, C. (1950). On war. Jazzybee Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language [Thinking and speeking]. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1974 [1925]). The psychology of art. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1994 [1934]). The problem of the environment. In R. van der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 338–354). Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watzlawick, P., et al. (1967). Pragmatics of human communication. A study of interactional patterns, pathologies, and paradoxes. W. W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wigfield, A. (1994). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation: A developmental perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 6(1), 49–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winnicott, D. W. (1991). Playing and reality. Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zittoun, T. (2013). On the use of a film: Cultural experiences as symbolic resources. In Little madnesses: Winnicott, transitional phenomena and cultural experience (pp. 135–147). I.B. Tauris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Žižek, S. (2004). Mapping ideology. Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Žižek, S. (2006). The parallax view. MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Žižek, S. (2011). How to read Lacan. Granta Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuboff, S. (2019). The age of surveillance capitalism: The fight for a human future at the new frontier of power: Barack Obama’s books of 2019. Profile books.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2023 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Nissen, M. (2023). A Post-psychology of Motivation. In: Rearticulating Motives. Theory and History in the Human and Social Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-43494-5_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics