Skip to main content

Contemporary Cognitive Approaches to Gender Development: New Schemas, New Directions, and New Conceptualizations of Gender

Part of the Focus on Sexuality Research book series (FOSR)

Abstract

The focus of this chapter is on two decades of advances in theory and research involving cognitive theories of gender development. The primary ways in which contemporary research has built upon previous work include (a) expanding the measurement and conceptualization of gender identities, (b) broadening the scope of gender-related cognitive constructs, and (c) detailing more closely the processes underlying gender development and describing the interrelations among these processes. In this chapter, we review the major themes underlying cognitive approaches, detail recent advances in research involving cognitive theories of gender development, and provide suggestions for future work that will capitalize on these advances.

Keywords

  • Gender identity
  • Gender schema
  • Cognitive development
  • Gender development
  • Cognitive theories
  • Identity development

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Aboud, F., & Brown, C. S. (2013). Positive and negative intergroup contact among children and its effect on attitudes. In G. Hudson & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Advances in intergroup contact (pp. 176–199). Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychological Association (APA). (2007). APA guidelines for psychological practice with girls and women. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/practice/guidelines/girls-and-women.aspx

  • American Psychological Association (APA), Boys and Men Guidelines Group. (2018). APA guidelines for psychological practice with boys and men. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/about/policy/boys-men-practice-guidelines.pdf

  • Andrews, N. C. Z., Martin, C. L., Field, R. D., Cook, R. E., & Lee, J. (2016). Development of expectancies of gender-based social exclusion and inclusion and their school-related consequences. Child Development, 87, 1423–1435.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, N. C. Z., Martin, C. L., Cook, R. E., Field, R. D., & England, D. E. (2019). Exploring dual gender typicality among young adults in the United States. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 43, 314–321.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Aoyagi, K., Santos, C. E., & Updegraff, K. A. (2018). Longitudinal associations between gender and ethnic-racial identity felt pressure from family and peers and self-esteem among African American and Latino/a youth. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47, 207–221.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354–364.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1983). Gender schema theory and its implications for child development: Raising gender-aschematic children in a gender- schematic society. Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 8, 598–616.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender: Transforming the debate on sexual inequality. Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Hines, M. (1992). Early androgens are related to childhood sex-typed toy preferences. Psychological Science, 3, 203–206.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., & Snyder, E. (1995). Early hormonal influences on childhood sex-typed activity and playmate preferences: Implications for the development of sexual orientation. Developmental Psychology, 31, 31–42.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Berenbaum, S. A., Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2008). Gender development. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.), Advances in child and adolescent development (pp. 647–696). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1992). Cognitive mechanisms in children’s gender stereotyping: Theoretical and educational implications of a cognitive-based intervention. Child Development, 63, 1351–1363.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). A cognitive-developmental approach to racial stereotyping and reconstructive memory in euro-American children. Child Development, 64, 1507–1519.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2006). A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 34, 39–89.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: Explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 162–166.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bigler, R. S., Jones, L. C., & Lobliner, D. B. (1997). Social categorization and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 68, 530–543.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradbard, M. R., Martin, C. L., Endsley, R. C., & Halverson, C. F. (1986). Influence of sex stereotypes on children’s exploration and memory: A competence versus performance distinction. Developmental Psychology, 22, 481–486.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C. S. (2017). Discrimination in childhood and adolescence: A developmental intergroup approach. Psychology Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2004). Children’s perceptions of gender discrimination. Developmental Psychology, 40, 714–726.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C. S., Bigler, R. S., & Chu, H. (2010). An experimental study of the correlates and consequences of perceiving oneself to be the target of gender discrimination. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 100–117.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bukowski, W. H., Gauze, C., Hoza, B., & Newcomb, A. F. (1993). Differences and consistency between same-sex and other-sex peer relationships during early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 29, 255–263.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106, 676–713.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, R. E., Nielson, M. G., Martin, C. L., & DeLay, D. (2019). Early adolescent gender development: The differential effects of felt pressure from parents, peers, and the self. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48, 1912–1923.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cvencek, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). Measuring implicit attitudes of 4-year-olds: The preschool implicit association test. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109, 187–200.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cvencek, D., Greenwald, A. G., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2016). Implicit measures for preschool children confirm self-esteem’s role in maintaining a balanced identity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 62, 50–57.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Del Giudice, M. (in press). Measuring sex differences and similarities. In D. P. VanderLaan & W. I. Wong (Eds.), Gender and sexuality development: Contemporary theory and research. Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeLay, D., Hanish, L. D., Zhang, L., & Martin, C. L. (2017). Assessing the impact of homophobic name calling on early adolescent mental health: A longitudinal social network analysis of competing peer influence effects. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46, 955–969.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeLucia, L. A. (1963). The toy preference test: A measure of sex role identification. Child Development, 34, 107–117.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. G. (2001). Gender identity: A multidimensional analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 37, 451–463.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrensaft, D. (2007). Raising Girlyboys: A Parent’s perspective. Studies in Gender and Sexuality, 8, 269–302.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Ehrensaft, D. (2011). Gender born, gender made. The Experiment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehrensaft, D. (2013). Look, mom, I’m a boy - don’t tell anyone I was a girl. Journal of LGBT Youth, 10, 9–28.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Endendijk, J. J., Beltz, A. M., McHale, S. M., Bryk, K., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2016). Linking prenatal androgens to gender-related attitudes, identity, and activities: Evidence from girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 45, 1807–1815.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Endendijk, J. J., Andrews, N. C. Z., England, D. E., & Martin, C. L. (2018). Gender-identity typologies are related to gender-typing, friendships, and social-emotional adjustment in emerging adults. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 43, 322–333.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, T., & Leaper, C. (2016). Chivalry’s double-edged sword: How girls’ and boys’ paternalistic attitudes relate to their possible family and work selves. Sex Roles, 74, 220–230.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Fast, A. A., & Olson, K. R. (2018). Gender development in transgender preschool children. Child Development, 89, 620–637.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Field, R. D., Martin, C. L., Andrews, D. W., England, D. E., & Zosuls, K. M. (2017). The influence of gender-based relationship efficacy on attitudes toward school. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 63, 396–422.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Ghavami, N., & Peplau, L. A. (2013). An intersectional analysis of gender and ethnic stereotypes: Testing three hypotheses. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37, 113–127.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Ghavami, N., & Peplau, L. A. (2018). Urban middle school students’ stereotypes at the intersection of sexual orientation, ethnicity, and gender. Child Development, 89, 881–896.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbons, J. L. (2000). Gender development in cross-cultural perspective. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 389–416). Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golombok, S., Rust, J., Zervoulis, K., Croudace, T., Golding, J., & Hines, M. (2008). Developmental trajectories of sex-typed behavior in boys and girls: A longitudinal general population study of children aged 2.5–8 years. Child Development, 79, 1583–1593.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gülgöz, S., Glazier, J. J., Enright, E. A., Alonso, D. J., Durwood, L. J., Fast, A. A., Lowe, R., Ji, C., Heer, J., Martin, C. L., & Olson, K. R. (2019). Gender development in transgender and cisgender children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 24480–24485.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Halim, M. D. (2016). Pink princesses and strong superheroes: Gender rigidity in early childhood. Child Development Perspectives, 10, 155–160.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Halim, M. L., Ruble, D., Tamis-LeMonda, C., & Shrout, P. E. (2013). Rigidity in gender-typed behaviors in early childhood: A longitudinal study of ethnic minority children. Child Development, 84, 1269–1284.

    CrossRef  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Halim, M. L., Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Baeg, S. A., Walsh, A., & Moy, K. H. (2015). Children’s dynamic gender identities across development and the influence of cognition, context, and culture. In Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (3rd ed., pp. 193–218). Taylor and Francis Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hidalgo, M. A., Ehrensaft, D., Tishelman, A. C., Clark, L. F., Garofalo, R., Rosenthal, S. M., Spack, N. P., & Olson, J. (2013). The gender affirmative model: What we know and what we aim to learn. Human Development, 56, 285–290.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M. (2015). Gendered development, Ch. 20. In R. M. Learner & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Socioemotional processes (7th ed.). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hines, M., Pasterski, V., Spencer, D., Neufeld, S., Patalay, P., Hindmarsh, P. C., … Acerini, C. L. (2016). Prenatal androgen exposure alters girls’ responses to information indicating gender-appropriate behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150125. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0125

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Huston, A. C. (1983). Sex-typing. In E. M. Hetherington & P. H. Mussen (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and social development (pp. 387–486). Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & van Anders, S. M. (2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five challenges to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74, 171–193.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jewell, J. A., & Brown, C. S. (2014). Relations among gender typicality, peer relations, and mental health during early adolescence. Social Development, 23, 137–156.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Kohlberg, L. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In E. E. Maccoby (Ed.), The development of sex differences (pp. 82–173). Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornienko, O., Santos, C. E., Martin, C. L., & Granger, K. L. (2016). Peer influence on gender identity development in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 52, 1578–1592.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kwan, K. M. W., Shi, S. Y., Nabbijohn, A. N., MacMullin, L. N., VanderLaan, D. P., & Wong, W. I. (2020). Children’s appraisals of gender nonconformity: Developmental pattern and intervention. Child Development, 91, 780–798.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (1994). Exploring the consequences of gender segregation on social relationships. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 65, 67–86.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (2011a). Research in developmental psychology on gender and relationships: Reflections on the past and looking into the future. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 347–356.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (2011b). More similarities than differences in contemporary theories of social development?: A plea for theory bridging. In J. B. Benson (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 337–378). Elsevier.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (2014). Gender similarities and differences in language. In T. M. Holtgraves (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of language and social psychology (pp. 62–81). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C. (2015). Gender and social-cognitive development. In L. S. Liben, U. Müller, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes (pp. 806–853). John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., & Brown, C. S. (2008). Perceived experiences with sexism among adolescent girls. Child Development, 79, 685–704.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leaper, C., Farkas, T., & Brown, C. S. (2012). Adolescent girls’ experiences and gender-related beliefs in relation to their motivation in math/science and English. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 268–282.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation: Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluating constructs and pathways. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67, vii–147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lytton, H., & Romney, D. M. (1991). Parents’ differential socialization of boys and girls: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 267–296.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (Ed.). (1966). The development of sex differences. Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Harvard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., & Jacklin, C. N. (1987). Gender segregation in children. In H. W. Reece (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 239–287). Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (1989). Children’s use of gender-related information in making social judgments. Developmental Psychology, 25, 80–88.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L. (2000). Cognitive theories of gender development. In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 91–122). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Dinella, L. M. (2012). Congruence between gender stereotypes and activity preference in self-identified tomboys and non-tomboys. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 41, 599–610.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Fabes, R. A. (2001). The stability and consequences of young children’s same-sex peer interactions. Developmental Psychology, 37, 431–446.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1981). A schematic processing model of sex typing and stereotyping in children. Child Development, 52, 1119–1134.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Halverson, C. F. (1983). Gender constancy: A methodological and theoretical analysis. Sex Roles, 9, 775–790.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 67–70.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Eisenbud, L., & Rose, H. A. (1995). Children’s gender-based reasoning about toys. Child Development, 66, 1453–1471.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Fabes, R. A., Evans, S., & Wyman, H. (1999). Social cognition on the playground: Children’s beliefs about playing with girls versus boys and their relations to sex segregated play. Journal of Personal and Social Relationships, 16, 751–771.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 903–933.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2004). Recognizing the centrality of gender identity and stereotype knowledge in gender development and moving toward theoretical integration: Reply to Bandura and Bussey. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 702–710.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Kornienko, O., Schaefer, D. R., Hanish, L. D., Fabes, R. A., & Goble, P. (2013). The role of sex of peers and gender-typed activities in young children’s peer affiliative networks: A longitudinal analysis of selection and influence. Child Development, 84, 921–937.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Andrews, N. C., England, D. E., Zosuls, K., & Ruble, D. N. (2017a). A dual identity approach for conceptualizing and measuring children’s gender identity. Child Development, 88, 167–182.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, C. L., Cook, R. E., & Andrews, N. C. Z. (2017b). Reviving androgyny: A modern day perspective on flexibility of gender identity and behavior. Sex Roles, 76, 592–603.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Master, A., & Walton, G. M. (2013). Minimal groups increase young children’s motivation and learning on group-relevant tasks. Child Development, 84, 737–751.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McKenney, S. J., & Bigler, R. S. (2014). High heels, low grades: Internalized sexualization and academic orientation among adolescent girls. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26, 30–36.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mehta, C. M., & Dementieva, Y. (2017). The contextual specificity of gender: Femininity and masculinity in college students’ same-and other-gender peer contexts. Sex Roles, 76, 604–614.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Menon, M. (2017). Multidimensional gender identity and gender-typed relationship styles in adolescence. Sex Roles, 76, 579–591.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W. (1966). A social learning view of sex differences in behavior. In E. Maccoby (Ed.), Development of sex differences (pp. 56–81). Stanford University Press. Google Scholar.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulvey, K. L., & Killen, M. (2015). Challenging gender stereotypes: Resistance and exclusion. Child Development, 86, 681–694.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nielson, M. G., Schroeder, K. M., Martin, C. L., & Cook, R. E. (2020). Investigating the relation between gender typicality and pressure to conform to gender norms. Sex Roles, 3, 523–535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, K. R., Durwood, L., DeMeules, M., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2016). Mental health of transgender children who are supported in their identities. Pediatrics, 137(3), e20153223. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-3223

    CrossRef  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Pahlke, E., Bigler, R. S., & Martin, C. L. (2014). Can fostering children’s ability to challenge sexism improve critical analysis, internalization, and enactment of inclusive, egalitarian peer relationships? Journal of Social Issues, 70, 115–133.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Pauletti, R. E., Cooper, P. J., & Perry, D. G. (2014). Influences of gender identity on children’s maltreatment of gender-nonconforming peers: A person × target analysis of aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 843–866.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pauletti, R. E., Menon, M., Cooper, P. J., Aults, C. D., & Perry, D. G. (2017). Psychological androgyny and children’s mental health: A new look with new measures. Sex Roles, 76, 705–718.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, D. G., Pauletti, R. E., & Cooper, P. J. (2019). Gender identity in childhood: A review of the literature. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 43, 289–304.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. Basic Books.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, A. A., DeLay, D., & Martin, C. L. (2017). Traditional masculinity during the middle school transition: Associations with depressive symptoms and academic engagement. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46, 709–724.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, A. J., Glick, G. G., & Smith, R. L. (2011). Popularity and gender: The culture of boys and girls. In A. H. N. Cillessen, D. Schwartz, & L. Mayeux (Eds.), Popularity in the peer system (pp. 103–122). Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Balaban, T., & Cooper, J. (1981). Gender constancy and the effects of sex-typed televised toy commercials. Child Development, 52, 667–673.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D. N., Taylor, L. J., Cyphers, L., Greulich, F. K., Lurye, L. E., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). The role of gender constancy in early gender development. Child Development, 78, 1121–1136.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schell, R. E., & Silber, J. W. (1968). Sex role discrimination among young children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 27, 379–389.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schubert, T. W., & Otten, S. (2002). Overlap of self, in-group, and out-group: Pictorial measures of self-categorization. Self and Identity, 1, 535–576.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Shutts, K., Banaji, M. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2010). Social categories guide young children’s preferences for novel objects. Developmental Science, 13, 599–610.

    CrossRef  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Signorella, M. L., Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (1993). Developmental differences in children’s gender schemata about others: A meta-analytic review. Developmental Review, 13, 147–183.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Stangor, C., & Ruble, D. N. (1987). Development of gender role knowledge and gender constancy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1987, 5–22.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Tam, M. J., Jewell, J. A., & Brown, C. S. (2019). Gender-based harassment in early adolescence: Group and individual predictors of perpetration. Applied Developmental Psychology, 62, 231–238.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Tate, C. C., Ledbetter, J. N., & Youssef, C. P. (2013). A two-question method for assessing gender categories in the social and medical sciences. Journal of Sex Research, 50, 767–776.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tate, C. C., Youssef, C. P., & Bettergarcia, J. N. (2014). Integrating the study of transgender spectrum and cisgender experiences of self-categorization from a personality perspective. Review of General Psychology, 18, 302–312.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Tobin, D. D., Menon, M., Menon, M., Spatta, B. C., Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (2010). The intrapsychics of gender: A model of self socialization. Psychological Review, 117, 601–622.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Trautner, H. M., Ruble, D. N., Cyphers, L., Kirsten, B., Behrendt, R., & Hartmann, P. (2005). Rigidity and flexibility of gender stereotypes in childhood: Developmental or differential? Infant and Child Development, 14, 365–380.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Tropp, L. R., & Wright, S. C. (2001). Ingroup identification as the inclusion of ingroup in the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 585–600.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • van Anders, S. M. (2015). Beyond sexual orientation: Integrating gender/sex and diverse sexualities via sexual configurations theory. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 44, 1177–1213.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Walby, S. (1990). Theorizing patriarchy. Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, G. M., Cohen, G. L., Cwir, D., & Spencer, S. J. (2012). Mere belonging: The power of social connections. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 513–532.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weisgram, E. S., Watson, E., & Zmudzinski, C. (2009, April). A test of the personal pathways model of gender differentiation. Paper presented at the Society for Research in child development, Denver, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wong, W. I., & VanderLaan, D. P. (2020). Early sex differences and similarities: Evidence across cultures? In F. M. Cheung & D. F. Halpern (Eds.), Cambridge international handbook on psychology of women. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xiao, S. X., Cook, R. E., Martin, C. L., Nielson, G. M., & Field, R. D. (2019). Will they listen to me? An examination of in-group gender bias in children’s communication beliefs. Sex Roles, 80, 172–185.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, C., Zuo, X., Blum, R. W., Tolman, D. L., Kågesten, A., Mmari, K., De Meyer, S., Michielsen, K., Basu, S., Acharya, R., & Lou, C. (2017). Marching to a different drummer: A cross-cultural comparison of young adolescents who challenge gender norms. Journal of Adolescent Health, 61, 48–54.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Yunger, J. L., Carver, P. R., & Perry, D. G. (2004). Does gender identity influence children’s psychological well-being? Developmental Psychology, 40, 572–582.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zosuls, K. M., Ruble, D. N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Shrout, P. E., Bornstein, M. H., & Greulich, F. K. (2009). The acquisition of gender labels in infancy: Implications for sex-typed play. Developmental Psychology, 45, 688–701.

    CrossRef  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Zosuls, K. M., Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., Miller, C., Gaertner, B. M., England, D. E., & Hill, A. P. (2011). “It’s not that we hate you”: Understanding children’s gender attitudes and expectancies about peer relationships. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 288–304.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zosuls, K. M., Field, R. D., Martin, C. L., Andrews, N. C., & England, D. E. (2014). Gender-based relationship efficacy: Children’s self-perceptions in intergroup contexts. Child Development, 85, 1663–1676.

    CrossRef  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zucker, K. J., & Bradley, S. J. (1995). Gender identity disorder and psychosexual problems in children and adolescents. Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rachel E. Cook .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Spotlight Feature: The Malleability of Gender: Conceptualizing Gender as a Contextual Variable

Spotlight Feature: The Malleability of Gender: Conceptualizing Gender as a Contextual Variable

Theoretical and conceptual models of gender as contextually malleable have been around since the late 1980s. Deaux and Major (1987) were the first psychologists studying gender to propose a contextual model of gender. Their contextual model promoted a social constructivist approach to gender and posited that rather than reflecting an individual’s personality traits, all gendered attributes including gendered attitudes, behaviors, femininity, and masculinity reflect an individual’s interaction with their immediate context (Deaux & Major, 1987, 1998). Viewed this way, gender is dynamic, context dependent, and continuously enacted, changing across time, relationships, and social context (Deaux & Major, 1987, 1998; Leszczynski & Strough, 2008; Shields, 1998).

While this theoretical work has undoubtedly contributed to psychologists’ understanding of gender as contextually dependent, social constructivist theory has not substantially shaped how researchers conceptualize and study gender (Mehta, 2015) and many psychologists studying gender continue to largely conceptualize gender as a single aspect of personality that remains stable across time and contexts. There are, however, a few exceptions (e.g., Cota & Dion, 1986; Pickard & Strough, 2003; Leszczynski & Strough, 2008). Below, we briefly review research studies that have conceptualized and investigated femininity/communion and masculinity/agency as contextually malleable, state variables.

Pickard and Strough (2003) modified the Bem Sex Role Inventory, a measure of stereotypical gender-typed traits (Bem, 1978) to investigate variations in femininity/communion and masculinity/agency in a sample of college students. Specifically, they asked students to rate the extent to which they currently identified with feminine/communal and masculine/agentic adjectives (e.g., “I currently feel nurturing”; “I currently feel aggressive”) both before and after interacting with a same-gender peer and an other-gender peer. They found that participants endorsed more feminine/communal adjectives after playing Jenga® (a turn-taking game in which blocks are removed from a tower) with an other-gender confederate than after playing Jenga® with a same-gender confederate. Additionally, male and female’s self-endorsement of masculine/agentic adjectives did not vary by context.

Using the same methodology, Leszczynski and Strough (2008) replicated these findings in a sample of young adolescents. In this study, both female and male adolescents endorsed more feminine/communal adjectives after playing Jenga® with a female confederate than after playing Jenga® with a male confederate. It could be that some aspects of gender are responsive to situational demands. For example, stereotypical beliefs about appropriate ways of interacting with women (e.g., being polite, collaborative) may have led participants to endorse femininity/communion to a greater extent when interacting with females. In this sample, masculinity/agency once again remained constant across partners. This may reflect that Jenga® elicits masculinity/agency, as even when played collaboratively, the game is competitive as players seek to win.

While these experimental studies have made important contributions to our understanding of femininity/communion and masculinity/agency as contextually modifiable state variables, they are limited in that they have low ecological validity and do not tell us much about how gender varies outside of a research lab, in people’s real-life contexts. To address this limitation, my research group employed Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) to study contextual variations in femininity/communion and masculinity/agency. EMA is a research methodology that allows for the repeated measurement of highly variable phenomena across time and contexts (Larson & Richards, 1994). Participants in EMA studies are prompted at random time points across a day to respond to a survey using a smartphone or other digital device (Mehta et al., 2014). Using this methodology, we are able to assess participants’ real-time endorsement of masculinity/agency and femininity/communion in their daily contexts, increasing ecological validity and reducing recall bias (Larson & Richards, 1994).

We used EMA to conduct a 2-week study investigating contextual variation in endorsement of femininity/communion and masculinity/agency. We found that femininity/communion and masculinity/agency varied over the course of the study. These variations were associated with social context. Specifically, men endorsed femininity/communion to a greater extent when they were in the company of women in comparison to when they were in the company of men. Men also reported greater masculinity when they were in the company of men (Mehta & Dementieva, 2017). It may be that masculinity norms—that encourage men to reject stereotypically feminine activities and behaviors (Bosson & Michniewicz, 2013)—lead men to exhibit and endorse low levels of femininity/communion and higher levels of masculinity when they are with other men (Mehta & Dementieva, 2017). Men may endorse greater femininity/communion when they are with women because they feel less pressure to conform to male gender role norms in these contexts (Werking, 1997).

There was no difference in women’s endorsement of femininity across female and male contexts, a finding that may be explained by our femininity measure picking up on a general activation of a socialized general communal/relational and cooperative orientation for women that exists across contexts (Mehta & Dementieva, 2017). Women did, however, report greater masculinity when they were in the company of men, perhaps reflecting women’s desire to build their own status by endorsing characteristics associated with a higher status gender group (Mehta & Dementieva, 2017).

To conclude, I believe that whether gender-related variables are stable or vary across time and context has important implications for the study of gender. In this brief spotlight feature, we have reviewed research that has examined variation in femininity/communion and masculinity/agency across the gender composition of people’s social contexts. However, other types of contexts such as physical context (e.g., home, school, and work) and activity context (playing competitive or cooperative games, volunteering, negotiating, etc.) should be considered. Researchers are often unable to find reliable and reproducible gender differences across a number of gender-related constructs. This is likely to be because many gender-related constructs are not reproducible as they vary across time and space. Consequently, if we do not incorporate context into gender research, our understanding of gender is likely to be incomplete. Furthermore, whether gender-related variables are stable or are contextually malleable has important implications for our broader societal understanding of gender. Shields (2013) describes a reciprocal cycle in which essentialized beliefs about gender differences, based on conceptualizations of gender-related variables as stable, are popularized by the media. The media in turn influences scientific enquiry, which then informs the media. By demonstrating that gender-related variables are not in fact innate pre-determined traits, but rather states that vary according to context, research that can illustrate the contextual specificity of gender-related constructs has the potential to break the cycle of gender essentialism that serves to underscore exaggerated societal beliefs about gender differences (e.g., Hyde, 2005).

Spotlight references

  • Bem, S. L. (1978). The Bem sex role inventory. Consulting Psychologist Press, Inc. Palo Alto, California.

  • Bosson, J. K., & Michniewicz, K. S. (2013). Gender dichotomization at the level of ingroup identity: What it is, and why men use it more than women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 425–442.

  • Cota, A. A., & Dion, K. L. (1986). Salience of gender and sex composition of ad hoc groups: An experimental test of distinctiveness theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 770–776.

  • Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.

  • Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1998). Gender behavior in a social context a social-psychological model of gender. In D. L. Anselmi & A. L. Law (Eds.), Questions of gender, perspectives and paradoxes (pp. 367–376). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

  • Hyde, J. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 581–592.

  • Larson, R., & Richards, M. H. (1994). Divergent realities: The emotional lives of mothers, fathers, and adolescents. New York, NY, US: Basic Books.

  • Leszczynski, J. P., & Strough, J. (2008). The contextual specificity of masculinity and femininity in early adolescence. Social Development, 17, 719–736.

  • Mehta, C. M. (2015). Gender in context: Considering variability in Wood and Eagly’s traditions of gender identity. Sex Roles, 73, 490-496.

  • Mehta, C. M., & Dementieva, Y. (2017). The contextual specificity of gender: Femininity and masculinity in college students’ same-and other-gender peer contexts. Sex Roles, 76, 604–614.

  • Mehta, C. M., Walls, C., Blood, E. A., & Shrier, L. A. (2014). Associations between affect, context, and sexual desire in depressed young women. Journal of Sex Research, 51, 577–585.

  • Pickard, J., & Strough, J. (2003). The effects of same-sex and other-sex contexts on masculinity and femininity. Sex Roles, 48, 421–432.

  • Shields, S. A. (1998). Gender in the psychology of emotion: A selective research review. In D. L. Anselmi & A. L. Law (Eds.), Questions of gender, perspectives and paradoxes (pp. 376–390). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

  • Shields, S. A. (2013). Gender and emotion: What we think we know, what we need to know, and why it matters. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 37, 423–435.

  • Werking, K. (1997). We’re just good friends: Women and men in nonromantic relationships. New York: Guilford Press.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

Copyright information

© 2022 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Cook, R.E., Martin, C.L., Nielson, M.G., Xiao, S.X. (2022). Contemporary Cognitive Approaches to Gender Development: New Schemas, New Directions, and New Conceptualizations of Gender. In: VanderLaan, D.P., Wong, W.I. (eds) Gender and Sexuality Development. Focus on Sexuality Research. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-84273-4_5

Download citation