Skip to main content

Thinking Through Experiment: Enacting RNAi

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
A Cognitive Ethnography of Knowledge and Material Culture

Part of the book series: Culture, Mind, and Society ((CMAS))

  • 374 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter characterizes a series of events sampled from RNAi experiments. Here, the parasite’s biological development is functionally characterized by silencing the expression patterns of specific genes from the lice genome. The initiation and termination of RNAi is a complex affair, whose execution requires the choreography of many researchers and materials. This chapter looks at how these situations set up epistemically rewarding and meaningful interrelationships by enacting various kinds of representations. Examples include how checklists help orchestrate molecular manipulations; how bioinformatic tools act as powerful epistemic enhancers; how microinjections of RNAi in the wet lab are executed; termination of the experiment; and how biological materials and their accompanying data propagate in experimental pipeline. Through choreographies of enacted understanding, the community of skilled practitioners turn isolated material patterns into meaningful wholes. Conventional ways of thinking about experimental knowledge as a property of individual minds, break down when these scientific actions are scrutinized in detail.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alač, M. (2011). Handling digital brains: A laboratory study of multimodal semiotic interaction in the age of computers. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S. (1990). Cognitive foundations of natural history: Towards an anthropology of science. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, H. (2011). The philosophical grammar of scientific practice. International Studies in the Philosophy of Science, 25(3), 205–221. https://doi.org/10.1080/02698595.2011.605244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coopmans, C., Vertesi, J., Lynch, M. E., & Woolgar, S. (2014). Representation in scientific practice revisited. MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Andrade, R. G. (1995). The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellen, R. (2004). From ethno-science to science, or what the indigenous knowledge debate tells us about how scientists define their project. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 4(3), 409–450. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568537042484869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellen, R. (2006). The categorical impulse. Berghan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galison, P. (1987). How experiments end. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galison, P. (1997). Image and logic: A material culture of microphysics. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giere, R. N. (2002). Discussion note: Distributed cognition in epistemic cultures. Philosophy of Science, 69(4), 637–644. https://doi.org/10.1086/344627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith, P. (2013). Philosophy of biology. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 606–633. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1994.96.3.02a00100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(10), 1489–1522. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00096-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2013). The co-operative, transformative organization of human action and knowledge. Journal of Pragmatics, 46(1), 8–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2012.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hacking, I. (1992). The self-vindication of the laboratory sciences. In A. Pickering (Ed.), Science as practice and culture (pp. 29–63). University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hazlehurst, B., McMullen, C. K., & Gorman, P. N. (2007). Distributed cognition in the heart room: How situation awareness arises from coordinated communications during cardiac surgery. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 40(5), 539–551. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbi.2007.02.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heintz, C. (2007). Institutions as mechanisms of cultural evolution: Prospects of the epidemiological approach. Biological Theory, 2(3), 244–249. https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2007.2.3.244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helmreich, S. (2009). Alien ocean: Anthropological voyages in microbial seas. University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollan, J., Hutchins, E., & Kirsh, D. (2000). Distributed cognition: Toward a new foundation for human-computer interaction research. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction (TOCHI), 7(2), 174–196. https://doi.org/10.1145/353485.353487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, D. L. (2010). Science as a process: An evolutionary account of the social and conceptual development of science. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, P. (2004). Extending ourselves: Computational science, empiricism, and scientific method. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. MIT press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2010). Enaction, imagination, and insight. In J. Stewart, O. Gapenne, & E. A. Di Paolo (Eds.), Enaction: Towards a new paradigm for cognitive science (pp. 425–450). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (2014). Two views of cognition. Lecture, Cognitive Science 102b Cognitive Ethnography, UCSD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, R. E. (1994). Lords of the fly: Drosophila genetics and the experimental life. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muldoon, R. (2013). Diversity and the division of cognitive labor. Philosophy Compass, 8(2), 117–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2009). How do engineering scientists think? Model-based simulation in biomedical engineering research laboratories. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1(4), 730–757. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01032.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nersessian, N. J. (2012). Engineering concepts: The interplay between concept formation and modeling practices in bioengineering sciences. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 19(3), 222–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/10749039.2012.688232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberger, H. J. (1997). Toward a history of epistemic things: Synthesizing proteins in the test tube. Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberger, H. J. (2010). An epistemology of the concrete: Twentieth-century histories of life. Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schickore, J., & Steinle, F. (2006). Introduction: Revisiting the context distinction. In J. Schickore, & F. Steinle (Eds.), Revisiting discovery and justification: Historical and philosophical perspectives on the context distinction (pp. 7–19). Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shore, B. (1995). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • SLRC. (2018). Sea Lice research center (Annual Report 2018). University of Bergen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sormani, P., Alač, M., Bovet, A., & Greiffenhagen, C. (2016). Ethnomethodology, video analysis, and STS. In U. Felt, R. Fouché, C. A. Miller, & L. Smith-Doerr (Eds.), The handbook of science and technology studies (pp. 113–138). MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1985). On anthropological knowledge: Three essays. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining culture. Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, H. (2013). Life out of sequence: A data-driven history of bioinformatics. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strasser, B. J. (2012). Data-driven sciences: From wonder cabinets to electronic databases. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C, 43(1), 85–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J., Goodwin, C., & LeBaron, C. (2011). Embodied interaction: Language and body in the material world. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suchman, L. A. (2007). Human-machine reconfigurations: Plans and situated actions. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, E. O., & Lieberman, B. S. (2011). Phylogenetics: Theory and practice of phylogenetic systematics. Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mads Solberg .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Solberg, M. (2021). Thinking Through Experiment: Enacting RNAi. In: A Cognitive Ethnography of Knowledge and Material Culture. Culture, Mind, and Society. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-72511-2_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics