Skip to main content

Introduction

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Measurement across the Sciences

Abstract

It would be difficult to overstate the value and importance of measurement in nearly every aspect of society. Every time we purchase or eat food, take prescribed medicine, travel in a vehicle, use a phone or computer, or step inside a building, we place our trust in the results of measurements—and, for the most part, that trust seems well earned, and as such measurement is commonly associated with precision, accuracy, and overall trustworthiness. Against this backdrop, it seems little wonder that the human sciences have, since their inception, attempted to incorporate measurement into their activities as well. However, despite—or perhaps, to at least some extent, because of—the ubiquity of measurement-related concepts and discourse, there remains a remarkable lack of shared understanding of these concepts across (and often within) different fields, perhaps most visibly reflected in the vast array of proposed definitions of measurement itself. In addition to hampering communication across different disciplinary fields regarding shared methodological principles, such a lack of common understanding hints at the possibility that the same terms—“measurement,” “measurement result,” “measurement model,” etc.—are used with very different and possibly even incompatible meanings in different disciplines, with potentially disastrous results.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We use the term “human sciences” to refer to all scientific disciplines and activities concerned with the human mind and behavior, including not only psychology, but also sociology, anthropology, and disciplines of research concerned with particular activities such as education, health and medicine, economics, and organizations. Thus, the term is interpreted analogously with the term “physical sciences”, which refers to not only physics but also other disciplines concerned with physical phenomena, such as chemistry, biology, geology, and astronomy.

  2. 2.

    We will carefully distinguish here objects, concepts for understanding objects, and terms for designating objects, and some notation will support us in this: thus, for example, measurement (no delimiters), as an object, is understood via a concept <measurement> (angular brackets) and designated in English by the term “measurement” (double quotes). See Box 2.1 for a presentation of our terminological assumptions.

  3. 3.

    This position seems to be broadly accepted in the human sciences as well; for example, as put by Warren Torgerson, “measurement enables the tool of mathematics to be applied to science” (1958: p. 1).

  4. 4.

    Work in the psychophysical tradition helped establish a number of relationships between physical phenomena and sensations (e.g., Fullerton & Cattell, 1892; Thurstone, 1927) that are even today still foundational for fields such as audiology.

  5. 5.

    A more nuanced variation of this conclusion is that there are different kinds of measurement that share common properties; this was the conclusion reached in particular by Ludwik Finkelstein, who argued for a distinction between “strongly defined measurement” and “weakly defined measurement”, where the former “follows the paradigm of the physical sciences [and] is based on: (1) precisely defined empirical operations, (2) mapping on the real number line on which an operation of addition is defined, (3) well-formed theories for broad domains of knowledge”, and the latter is “measurement that [...] lacks some, or all, of the above distinctive characteristics of strong measurement” (Finkelstein, 2003: p. 42).

  6. 6.

    www.britannica.com/science/thermoreception.

  7. 7.

    www.britannica.com/science/temperature.

  8. 8.

    More typically, these would be called “points”: i.e., “100 points above the mean”.

  9. 9.

    The notation Θ[a] is introduced and explained in Footnote 15 of Chap. 2.

  10. 10.

    As in Fig. 4.6, whose axes highlight whether measurement has been characterized as being dependent on empirical and/or mathematical constraints, respectively.

References

  • Boumans, M. (2007). Invariance and calibration. In M. Boumans (Ed.), Measurement in economics: A handbook (pp. 231–248). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Briggs D. (in press). Credos and controversies: Historical and conceptual foundations of measurement in the human sciences. CRC Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bringmann, L. F., & Eronen, M. I. (2015). Heating up the measurement debate: What psychologists can learn from the history of physics. Theory & Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354315617253

  • Cameron, W. B. (1963). Informal sociology: A casual introduction to sociological thinking. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, N. R. (1920). Physics—The elements. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cartwright, N. (1999). The dappled world—A study of the boundaries of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, J. M. (1890). Mental tests and measurements. Mind, 15, 373–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, J. M., & Fullerton, G. S. (1892). The psychophysics of movement. Mind, 1(3), 447–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chang, H. (2007). Inventing temperature—Measurement and scientific progress. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edgeworth, F. Y. (1888). The statistics of examinations. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 51, 599–635.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edgeworth, F. Y. (1892). Correlated averages. Philosophical Magazine, 5th Series, 34, 190–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G. T. (1860/1966). Elements of psychophysics. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, A., Myers, C. S., Bartlett, R. J., Banister, H., Bartlett, F. C., Brown, W., et al. (1940). Final report of the committee appointed to consider and report upon the possibility of quantitative estimates of sensory events. Report of the British Association for the Advancement of Science, 2, 331–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finkelstein, L. (2003). Widely, strongly and weakly defined measurement. Measurement, 34, 39–48.

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, L. (1944). A basis for scaling qualitative data. American Sociological Review, 9, 139–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, E. J. (1916). The Kansas silent reading tests. Journal of Educational Psychology, 7, 63–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1961). The function of measurement in modern physical science. Isis, 52(2), 161–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mari, L., & Wilson, M. (2014). An introduction to the Rasch measurement approach for metrologists. Measurement, 51, 315–327.

    Article  ADS  Google Scholar 

  • McGrane, J. A. (2015). Stevens’ forgotten crossroads: The divergent measurement traditions in the physical and psychological sciences from the mid-twentieth century. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michell, J. (1999). Measurement in psychology: A critical history of a methodological concept. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nunnally, J. M., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherry, D. (2011). Thermoscopes, thermometers, and the foundations of measurement. Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A, 42, 509–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1904). The proof and measurement of association between two things. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 72–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spearman, C. (1907). Demonstration of formulae for true measurement of correlation. American Journal of Psychology, 18, 161–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thurstone, L. L. (1927). A law of comparative judgment. Psychological Review, 34, 273–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgerson, W. S. (1958). Theory and methods of scaling. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Mari, L., Wilson, M., Maul, A. (2021). Introduction. In: Measurement across the Sciences. Springer Series in Measurement Science and Technology. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-65558-7_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics