Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Language, Cognition, and Mind ((LCAM,volume 8))

  • 279 Accesses

Abstract

There is a Buddhist story which begins with a Buddhist master and his disciple receiving a guest who talked on a certain topic and the master agreeing with what the guest had said.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The cousins of reflexive pronouns, namely pronouns and r-expressions, are not directly discussed, as such a discussion would go far beyond the scope of this book.

  2. 2.

    This theoretical grounding is itself rooted in Givón’s (1979) original pragmatic versus syntactic communication mode of distinction; see also Givón (2018) for terminological updates.

  3. 3.

    A reviewer notes that we should not forget other languages, such as Icelandic, which show LD binding phenomena (which can be captured syntactically). So it is not claimed that a language showing LD binding is automatically assigned to a pragmatic language group—it is necessary to discover what the mechanisms are behind the LD binding, and also to assess the language on the other factors within the syntactic-pragmatic continuum.

  4. 4.

    I leave aside the assessment of their ‘now-or-never bottleneck’, and how it might be applied to binding. See Christiansen and Chater (2016) for extensive discussion and commentary.

  5. 5.

    Extending Christiansen and Chater’s (2016) idea of Chomsky’s hidden legacy, which discusses the general assumption made by many that there exists a competence-performance distinction, there is another legacy whereby many researchers claim that anaphoric dependencies are purely syntactic in nature. This assumption is certainly challenged here and occasionally acknowledged by proponents of the pure syntax model. For instance, a reviewer’s comment on a pure-syntactic analysis proposed for the Japanese reflexive pronoun zibun by Nishigauchi (2014: 204) prompted the latter to say, “It is certainly true that it is not a priori the case that replacing pragmatic or functional accounts by syntactic ones means progress in science”.

  6. 6.

    It is important to note that reflexivity can be expressed in many different ways (see Faltz (1985) for a survey), and standalone reflexive pronouns are but a subcategory.

  7. 7.

    It should be noted that there is a tight relationship between reflexives and reciprocals in some languages (such as German)—for discussion see Gast and Haas (2008).

  8. 8.

    Siemund (2000) discussed their inclusive/exclusive use, König and Siemund (2000) examined the emphatic and intensifier use, Gast and Siemund (2006) studied the contrastive use. See also Kemmer (1995) for comparisons between reflexive and emphatic –self in English.

  9. 9.

    ‘General’ is the key as this does not hold for all languages. Some languages use ‘head’ or ‘body’ morphemes for the reflexive component (cf. Faltz (1985) for further discussion).

  10. 10.

    It is noted here that this does not make these particular reflexives complex in Chinese, Japanese and Korean. This is discussed further on in Chap. 2.

  11. 11.

    This was previously adopted by Steinbach (2002a).

  12. 12.

    This restriction does not apply to the term ‘anaphora’, which depending on what is discussed, may refer to different groupings (cf. Huang 2000: 2–5).

  13. 13.

    A reviewer points out that this is not necessarily the case across all languages, for example considering binding patterns in Icelandic and Faroese (cf. Reuland 2011).

  14. 14.

    This type of sentence is problematic for the Binding theory, as it assumes that anaphors and pronouns are in complementary distribution (and where a reflexive occurs one will not find a pronoun). In any case, the fact that both forms can have John as their antecedent without any difference in structure occurring suggests a difference in meaning. For example, the perspective taken by the speaker has subtle effects on the pronominal selected (Kuno 1987).

  15. 15.

    See also Charnavel (2019) for further discussion on how logophoricity interacts with non-argument reflexives in English.

  16. 16.

    English does have standalone self available, sometimes used in news headlines, such as Cannibal eats self. Its usage is highly restricted.

  17. 17.

    The only difference would be that taziji is de facto gender-neutral in speech (他), and in writing as well. However, ta also has a feminine form 她.

  18. 18.

    See Huang (2000: 98–99) for data on split antecedence in different languages.

  19. 19.

    Experimentally, word order can be taken advantage of. In the SVO order, once the verb is met, this can lead predictions, but in the SOV order the later occurrence of the verb cannot be relied upon in the same way.

  20. 20.

    This also may appear in adjunct PPs, possessive NPs and emphatic NPs (Huang 2000: 22–23).

  21. 21.

    One problem with discussing finiteness is that languages like Chinese do not overtly display any finiteness distinction, suggesting that it does not exist, which is heavily debated (Hu et al. 2001; Sybesma 2007; Lin 2010; T.-H. J. Lin 2011; Lin 2015; Huang 2018). The possible positions on finiteness in Chinese are that there is no finiteness, finite only, non-finite only, or both (cf. Huang (1994: 40)). Pursuing the assumption that clauses are finite only seems to present the least amount of problems to syntax. See comments from Despić (2015) for TP-less Mandarin Chinese (also applied to Japanese and Korean).

  22. 22.

    See Sect. 2.14 for further discussion regarding Chinese. Japanese also exhibits ‘honorific’ blocking (Huang 2000: 121–122). For blocking effects in English see Sect. 2.11. See also H. Park (2016) for comments on Korean.

  23. 23.

    See Hulk and Cornips (2000), Steinbach (2002b) and Lekakou (2005a) for detailed treatment.

  24. 24.

    The statue and strict/sloppy interpretations in the languages studied here are mentioned in next chapter, but only in a general manner, as interpretations can be affected by structure type and context, requiring an in-depth assessment beyond the scope of this book.

References

  • Ariel, M. (1991). The functions of accessibility in a theory of grammar. Journal of Pragmatics, 16, 443–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, M. (1994). Interpreting anaphoric expressions: A cognitive versus a pragmatic approach. Journal of Linguistics, 30(1), 3–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, M. (2001). Accessibility theory: An overview. In T. Sanders, J. Schliperoord, & W. Spooren (Eds.), Text representation (pp. 29–87). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, M. (2008). Pragmatics and grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C., Hornstein, N., & Nunes, J. (2007). Overt copies in reflexive and control structures: a movement analysis. University of Maryland Working Papers in Linguistics 15, 1–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burzio, L. (1998). Anaphora and soft constraints. In P. Barbosa, D. Fox, P. Hagstrom, M. McGinnis, & D. Pesetsky (Eds.), Is the best good enough? Optimality and competition in syntax (pp. 93–113). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Charnavel, I. (2019). Locality and logophoricity. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, D.-I. (1996). Anaphor or pronominal? Language Research, 32(4), 621–636.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). The now-or-never bottleneck: a fundamental constraint on language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 39. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1500031X.

  • Despić, M. (2015). Phases, reflexives, and definiteness. Syntax, 3(18), 201–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dishcer, S. (2004). Optimal binding. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 22, 481–526.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drummond, A. (2011). Binding phenomena within a reductionist theory of grammatical dependencies. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Drummond, A., Kush, D., & Hornstein, N. (2011). Minimalist construal: Two approaches to A and B. In C. Boeckx (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic minimalism (pp. 396–426). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, N., & Levinson, S. C. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 429–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Faltz, L. M. (1985). Reflexivization: A study in universal syntax. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gast, V. (2006). The grammar of identity: Intensifiers and reflexives in Germanic languages. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gast, V., & Haas, F. (2008). On reciprocal and reflexive uses of anaphors in German and other European languages. In E. König & V. Gast (Eds.), Reciprocals and reflexives: Theoretical and typological explorations (pp. 308–346). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gast, V., & Siemund, P. (2006). Rethinking the relationship between SELF-intensifiers and reflexives. Linguistics, 44(2), 343–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, A. (2004). Long distance anaphors and temporal relations. In Proceedings of the 23rd West Coast conference on formal linguistics, eds. Vineeta Chand, Ann Kelleher, Angelo J. Rodríguez, and Benjamin Schmeiser, 223–236. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, A. (2006). From temporal anchoring to long distance anaphors. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 24, 1009–1047.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giorgi, A. (2007). On the nature of long-distance anaphors. Linguistic Inquiry, 38(2), 321–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Givón, T. (1979). On understanding grammar. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Givón, T. (2018). On understanding grammar (revised). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagège, C. (1974). Les pronoms logophoriques. Bulletin De La Société De Linguistique, 69, 287–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hornstein, N. (2000). Move! A minimalist theory of construal. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, Y. (1994). The syntax and pragmatics of anaphora: A study with special reference to Chinese. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, Y. (2000). Anaphora: A cross-linguistic study. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, Y. (2016). Aspects of anaphora in Chinese and in some Germanic, Romance, and Slavic languages, the ‘syntactic’ versus ‘pragmatic’ language typology, and neo-Gricean pragmatics. In K. Allan, A. Capone, & I. Kecskes (Eds.), Pragmemes and theories of language use (pp. 21–43). Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, N. (2018). Control complement in Mandarin Chinese: Implications for restructuring and the Chinese finiteness debate. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 27, 347–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulk, A., & Cornips, L. (2000). Reflexives in middles and the syntax-semantics interface. In H. Bennis, M. Everaert, & E. Reuland (Eds.), Interface strategies (pp. 207–222). Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hu, J., Pan, H., & Xu, L. (2001). Is there a finite vs. nonfinite distinction in Chinese? Linguistics 39(6), 1117–1148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1992). MME. Tussaud meets the binding theory. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 10, 1–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, E., Runner, J. T., Sussman, R. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2009). Structural and semantic constraints on the resolution of pronouns and reflexives. Cognition, 112, 55–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E. L. (2003). A historical explanation of some binding theoretic facts in English. In J. Moore & M. Polinsky (Eds.), The nature of explanation in linguistic theory (pp. 154–189). Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemmer, S. (1995). Emphatic and reflexive—self: Expectations, viewpoint, and subjectivity. In D. Stein & S. Wright (Eds.), Subjectivity and subjectivisation: Linguistic perspectives (pp. 55–82). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • König, E., & Siemund, P. (2000). Intensifiers and reflexives: A typological perspective. In Z. Frajzyngier & T. S. Curl (Eds.), Reflexives: Forms and functions (pp. 41–74). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuno, S. (1987). Functional syntax: Anaphora, discourse and empathy. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lederer, J. (2013). Understanding the self: How spatial parameters influence the distribution of anaphora within prepositional phrases. Cognitive Linguistics, 24(3), 483–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, C. (1988). Issues in Korean anaphora. In E.-J. Baek (Ed.), Papers from the 6th international conference on Korean linguistics (pp. 339–358). Seoul: Hanshin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lekakou, M. (2005a). In the middle, somewhat elevated: the semantics of middles and its crosslinguistic realization. PhD dissertation, University of London, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lidz, J. (2001). Anti-antilocality. In Syntax and semantics 33: long-distance reflexives, eds. Peter Cole, Gabriella Hermon, and C.-T. James Huang, 227–254. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, J.-W. (2010). A tenseless analysis of Mandarin Chinese revisited: A response to Sybesma 2007. Linguistic Inquiry, 41(2), 305–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, T.-H. (2011). Finiteness of clauses and raising of arguments in Mandarin Chinese. Syntax, 14(1), 48–73.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lin, T.-H. (2015). Tense in Mandarin Chinese sentences. Syntax, 18(3), 320–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (2005). Extending the competition model. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(7), 29–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mattausch, J. (2003). Optimality theoretic pragmatics and binding phenomena. In R. Blutner & H. Zeevat (Eds.), Optimality theory and pragmatics (pp. 63–90). London: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeown, R. K. (2013). A movement account of long-distance reflexives. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKillen, A. (2016). On the interpretation of reflexive pronouns. Ph.D. dissertation, McGill University, Montreal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishigauchi, T. (2014). Reflexive binding: Awareness and empathy from a syntactic point of view. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 23(2), 157–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Grady, W. (2005). Syntactic carpentry: An emergentist approach to syntax. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Okada, S. (1998). Reflexive pronouns with split antecedents. Journal of Pragmatics, 30, 59–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Park, H. (2016). Long-distance anaphors and the blocking effect revisited: an East Asian perspective. In Proceedings of the 30th Pacific Asia conference on language, information and computation, eds. Jong C. Park, and Jin-Woo Chung, 95–103. Seoul: Kyung Hee University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollard, C., & Sag, I. A. (1992). Anaphors in English and the scope of binding theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 23(2), 261–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollard, C., & Xue, P. (1998). Chinese reflexive ziji: syntactic reflexives vs. nonsyntactic reflexives. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 7 (4), 287–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, T., & Siloni, T. (2005). The lexicon-syntax parameter: Reflexivization and other arity operations. Linguistic Inquiry, 36(3), 389–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reuland, E. (2011). Anaphora and language design. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Safir, K. (2013). Syntax, binding, and patterns of anaphora. In M. den Dikken (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of generative syntax (pp. 515–576). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schladt, M. (2000). The typology and grammaticalization of reflexives. In Z. Frajzyngier & T. S. Curl (Eds.), Reflexives: Forms and functions (pp. 41–74). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siemund, P. (2000). Intensifiers in English and German: A comparison. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solberg, P. E. (2017). The discourse semantics of long-distance reflexives. PhD dissertation, University of Oslo, Oslo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spathas, G. (2010). Focus on reflexive anaphors. In Proceedings of SALT 20, eds. Nan Li, and David Lutz, 471–488. Ithaca: NY: CLC Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinbach, M. (2002a). Middle voice: A comparative study in the syntax-semantics interface of German. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinbach, M. (2002b). The ambiguity of weak reflexive pronouns in English and German. In Studies in comparative Germanic syntax: proceedings from the 15th workshop on comparative Germanic syntax eds. C. Jan-Wouter Zwart, and Werner Abraham, 317–342. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sybesma, R. (2007). Whether we tense-agree overtly or not. Linguistic Inquiry, 38(3), 580–587.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Gelderen, E. (2000). Bound pronouns and non-local anaphors. In Z. Frajzyngier & T. S. Curl (Eds.), Reflexives: Forms and functions (pp. 187–225). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Hoek, K. (1997). Anaphora and conceptual structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Darcy Sperlich .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Sperlich, D. (2020). A Beginning Synthesis. In: Reflexive Pronouns: A Theoretical and Experimental Synthesis. Language, Cognition, and Mind, vol 8. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-63875-7_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-63875-7_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-63874-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-63875-7

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics