Interpretive Play and the Player Psychology of Optimal Arousal Regulation
- 295 Downloads
Abstract
Building on previous discussions of interpretive play in story-focused digital games, a psychological foundation of narrative interpretation is proposed. First, how narrative information is synthesized with prior experiences and expectations into ‘situation models’ in long-term memory. Second, how pleasure is derived from increased arousal through engaging with novel, unfamiliar stimuli. This psychological foundation is then compared to contemporary approaches to narrative design, particularly in Her Story and Dear Esther. The relevance of the proposed cognitive psychological foundation is also considered in regard to digital games with no documented intentional use psychologically, primarily through an analysis of player behaviour and response in Gone Home. Results are comparable to the psychological model, particularly the prominent roles of recalling and forming expectations, and how player attention is often attracted to novel, unfamiliar, or unexpected stimuli. The ongoing aim of this work is to further investigate interpretive play in the context of digital games and continue to develop the cognitive psychological foundation. Future work shall also contextualise the research and findings though the development of commercial games.
Keywords
Interpretive play Arousal regulation Digital games Narrative Cognitive psychology SchemaReferences
- 1.Upton, B.: The Aesthetic of Play. MIT Press, Cambridge (2015)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 2.Bozdog, M., Galloway, D.: Worlds at our fingertips: reading (in) what remains of edith finch. Games Cult. 1555412019844631 (2019)Google Scholar
- 3.Sparrow, G.: What Remains of Edith Finch. Annapurna Interactive, Los Angeles (2017)Google Scholar
- 4.Boyd, B.: On the Origin of Stories. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (2009)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 5.The Chinese Room: Dear Esther. The Chinese Room, Curve Digital, Brighton, United Kingdom and London, United Kingdom (2012)Google Scholar
- 6.Barlow, S.: Her Story. Self-published (2015)Google Scholar
- 7.The Fullbright Company M.C.: Gone Home. The Fullbright Company and Majesco Entertainment, Portland (2013)Google Scholar
- 8.Baddeley, A.: Working memory. Science 255, 556–559 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 9.An, S.: Schema theory in reading. Theory Pract. Lang. Stud. 3 (2013)Google Scholar
- 10.Mandler, J.M.: Stories, Scripts, and Scenes: Aspects of Schema Theory. Psychology Press (2014)Google Scholar
- 11.Bartlett, F.C.: Remembering: An Experimental and Social Study. Cambridge University, Cambridge (1932)Google Scholar
- 12.Mercier, P., Kalampalikis, N.: Repeated reproduction: back to bartlett A French replication of narrative and an extension to proverbs. Cult. Psychol. 26(3), 500–527 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X19871197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 13.Rubínová, E., Blank, H., Koppel, J., Ost, J.: Schema and deviation effects in remembering repeated unfamiliar stories. Br. J. Psychol. (2020)Google Scholar
- 14.Howell, P.: Disruptive game design: a commercial design and development methodology for supporting player cognitive engagement in digital games. Creative Technologies, Doctor of Philosophy. University of Portsmouth (2015)Google Scholar
- 15.Howell, P.: A theoretical framework of ludic knowledge: a case study in disruption and cognitive engagement. In: 10th International Conference in the Philosophy of Computer Games, Valletta, Malta, October 2016Google Scholar
- 16.The Chinese Room: Amnesia: A Machine for Pigs. Frictional Games, Helsingborg (2013)Google Scholar
- 17.Douglas, J.Y., Hargadon, A.: The pleasures of immersion and engagement: schemas, scripts and the fifth business. Digit. Creat. 12, 153–166 (2001)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 18.Van Dijk, T.A., Kintsch, W.: Strategies of discourse comprehension (1983)Google Scholar
- 19.Zwaan, R.A., Radvansky, G.A.: Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychol. Bull. 123, 162 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 20.Zwaan, R.A., Langston, M.C., Graesser, A.C.: The construction of situation models in narrative comprehension: an event-indexing model. Psychol. Sci. 6, 292–297 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 21.Zacks, J.M., Speer, N.K., Swallow, K.M., Braver, T.S., Reynolds, J.R.: Event perception: a mind-brain perspective. Psychol. Bull. 133, 273 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 22.Zacks, J.M., Swallow, K.M.: Event segmentation. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 80–84 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 23.Newtson, D., Engquist, G.: The perceptual organization of ongoing behavior. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 12, 436–450 (1976)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 24.Dream, Q.: Detroit: Become Human. Sony Interactive Entertainment, San Mateo (2018)Google Scholar
- 25.Oatley, K.: A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative. Poetics 23, 53–74 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 26.Csikszentmihalyi, M., Csikzentmihaly, M.: Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. Harper & Row, New York (1990)Google Scholar
- 27.Oliver, M.B., Bartsch, A.: Appreciation of entertainment. J. Media Psychol. 23, 29–33 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 28.Cohen, A.R., Stotland, E., Wolfe, D.M.: An experimental investigation of need for cognition. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 51, 291 (1955)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 29.Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E.: The need for cognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 42, 116 (1982)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 30.Cacioppo, J.T., Petty, R.E., Feng Kao, C.: The efficient assessment of need for cognition. J. Personal. Assess. 48, 306–307 (1984)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 31.Litman, J.: Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: wanting and liking new information. Cogn. Emot. 19, 793–814 (2005)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 32.Edelman, S.: Curiosity and exploration (1997). Accessed 11 May 20Google Scholar
- 33.Litman, J.A., Jimerson, T.L.: The measurement of curiosity as a feeling of deprivation. J. Personal. Assess. 82, 147–157 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 34.Litman, J.A.: Interest and deprivation factors of epistemic curiosity. Personal. Individ. Differ. 44, 1585–1595 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 35.Costikyan, G.: Uncertainty in Games. MIT Press, Cambridge (2013)Google Scholar
- 36.To, A., Ali, S., Kaufman, G.F., Hammer, J.: Integrating curiosity and uncertainty in game design. In: Digra/fdg (2016)Google Scholar
- 37.Kruglanski, A.W., Webster, D.M.: Motivated closing of the mind: seizing and freezing. Psychol. Rev. 103, 263 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 38.Webster, D.M., Kruglanski, A.W.: Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 67, 1049 (1994)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 39.Hiel, A.V., Mervielde, I.: The need for closure and the spontaneous use of complex and simple cognitive structures. J. Soc. Psychol. 143, 559–568 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 40.Berlyne, D.E.: Aesthetics and Psychobiology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. Meredith Corporation, New York (1971)Google Scholar
- 41.Gaver, W.W., Mandler, G.: Play it again, sam: on liking music. Cogn. Emot. 1, 259–282 (1987)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 42.Reisenzein, R., Horstmann, G., Schützwohl, A.: The cognitive-evolutionary model of surprise: a review of the evidence. Top. Cogn. Sci. 11, 50–74 (2019)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 43.Nell, V.: Lost in a Book: The Psychology of Reading for Pleasure. Yale University Press, New Haven (1988)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 44.Matsubara, M., Augereau, O., Sanches, C.L., Kise, K.: Emotional arousal estimation while reading comics based on physiological signal analysis. In: Proceedings of the 1st International Workshop on coMics ANalysis, Processing and Understanding, pp. 1–4. (2016)Google Scholar
- 45.Gregersen, A., Langkjær, B., Heiselberg, L., Wieland, J.L.: Following the viewers: investigating television drama engagement through skin conductance measurements. Poetics 64, 1–13 (2017)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 46.Bhattacharjee, T., Datta, S., Das, D., Choudhury, A.D., Pal, A., Ghosh, P.K.: A heart rate driven kalman filter for continuous arousal trend monitoring. In: 2018 40th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), pp. 3572–3577. IEEE (2018)Google Scholar
- 47.Hirokawa, E.: Effects of music listening and relaxation instructions on arousal changes and the working memory task in older adults. J. Music Ther. 41, 107–127 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 48.Piaget, J.: Piaget’s Theory. Piaget and His School, pp. 11–23. Springer, Heidelberg (1976). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-46323-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 49.Brewer, W.F.: The nature of narrative suspense and the problem of rereading. In: Suspense: Conceptualizations, Theoretical Analyses, and Empirical Explorations, pp. 107–127 (1996)Google Scholar
- 50.Brewer, W.F., Lichtenstein, E.H.: Stories are to entertain: a structural-affect theory of stories. Center for the Study of Reading Technical Report; no. 265 (1982)Google Scholar
- 51.Truffaut, F., Hitchcock, A., Scott, H.G.: Hitchcock. Simon and Schuster (1984)Google Scholar
- 52.Smith, P.: Hemingway’s early manuscripts: the theory and practice of omission. J. Mod. Lit. 10, 268–288 (1983)Google Scholar
- 53.Barlow, S.: Telling Lies. Annapurna Interactive, California (2019)Google Scholar
- 54.Game Developers Conference. https://www.gdcvault.com/play/1023430/Making-Her-Story-Telling-a
- 55.Games, B.: Thirty Flights of Loving. Idle Thumbs, San Francisco (2012)Google Scholar
- 56.Variable State: Virginia. 505 Games, Milan, Italy (2016)Google Scholar
- 57.Pinchbeck, D., Stevens, B.: Presence, narrative and schemata. In: Proceedings of Presence 2005: The 8th International Workshop on Presence, pp. 221–226 (2005)Google Scholar
- 58.Pinchbeck, D., Stevens, B., Van Laar, S., Hand, S., Newman, K.: Narrative, agency and observational behaviour in a first person shooter environment. In: Proceedings of Narrative AI and Games Symposium: Society for the Study of Artificial Intelligence and the Simulation of Behaviour (AISOB 2006), pp. 53–61. SSAISB (2006)Google Scholar
- 59.Pinchbeck, D.: Story and recall in first-person shooters. Int. J. Comput. Games Technol. 2008, 6 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 60.Pinchbeck, D.: Dear esther: an interactive ghost story built using the source engine. In: Spierling, U., Szilas, N. (eds.) ICIDS 2008. LNCS, vol. 5334, pp. 51–54. Springer, Heidelberg (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-89454-4_9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 61.Game Developers ConferenceGoogle Scholar
- 62.Valve Corporation: Half-Life 2. Self-published, Bellevue (2004)Google Scholar
- 63.Pitkänen, J.: Studying thoughts: stimulated recall as a game research method. Game research methods, pp. 117–132 (2015)Google Scholar
- 64.Braun, V., Clarke, V.: Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- 65.Higgins, M.: White Lake. Self-published (2015)Google Scholar