Skip to main content

Music Archaeology, Signaling Theory, Social Differentiation

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Explorations in Archaeology and Philosophy

Part of the book series: Synthese Library ((SYLI,volume 433))

Abstract

Musical flutes (pipes) constructed from bird bone and mammoth ivory begin to appear in the archaeological record from around 40,000 years ago. Due to the different physical demands of acquiring and working with these source materials in order to produce a flute, researchers have speculated about the significance—aesthetic or otherwise—of the use of mammoth ivory as a raw material for flutes. I argue that biological signaling theory provides a theoretical basis for the proposition that mammoth ivory flute production is a signal of increasing social differentiation in Upper Palaeolithic human life.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    This chapter expands and develops an idea I suggested in Killin (2018). The basic idea follows Kim Sterelny’s argument that symbolic culture signals not an upgrade in individual cognitive sophistication or the first appearance of self-identifying collectives, but is both partial cause and effect of increasing social hierarchy and wealth differential (Sterelny 2012, pp. 52–54).

  2. 2.

    For example, see (hear) experimental archaeologist and flautist Fredrich Seeberger’s CD Klangwelten der Altsteinzeit (“Soundworld of the Old Stone Age”), for Urgeschichtliches Museum Blaubeuren. Seeberger recorded experimental/explorative performances of replicas of Upper Palaeolithic flutes in Hohle Fels cave, demonstrating a range of possibilities of these flutes in one of their presumed acoustic spaces.

  3. 3.

    For discussion within this framework of this and other relevant examples, see Pain (2019).

  4. 4.

    For philosophical treatments of signaling theory, see work by Brian Skyrms (e.g., Skyrms 2004, 2010) and Peter Godfrey-Smith (e.g., Godfrey-Smith 2014a, b).

  5. 5.

    Actual cases of complete common interest are of course rare. However, cases of partial common interest can be more or less aligned, and this graded conception is all we need.

  6. 6.

    Not all high-cost signals are hard-to-fake and vice versa; for reasons of exposition I do not explicitly distinguish these here as that is a task not without difficulties and is orthogonal to my aims. See Brusse (2019) for philosophical analysis.

  7. 7.

    Tattooing is an ancient practice at least several thousand years old, but there is no evidence of it in Palaeolithic humans, and in any case, “tattooing does not consume much pigment, so most of what we find was probably used for other purposes” (Kuhn 2014, p. 44).

  8. 8.

    The material record provides ample evidence of the big game hunting skills of Late Pleistocene humans. Even if the ivory was acquired through scavenging practices rather than active hunting it would still have to be extracted from the carcass, which is not effortless, and may well have involved deterring other predators or scavengers.

  9. 9.

    They may also have been a direct causal mechanism of that shift, though I have not made the case for that herein.

  10. 10.

    Moreover, it was only due to painstakingly intricate efforts at waterscreening, organizing, and refitting that many of the bone and ivory Upper Palaeolithic flutes have been reconstructed and identified. Other excavations may have recovered fragments that remain unidentified.

  11. 11.

    I write in the present tense even though some traditions may no longer be practiced as they used to be prior to Western influence and other modern-day factors.

  12. 12.

    Hunting bow technologies appear in the archaeological record from 64 kya (Lombard and Haidle 2011; Lombard and Phillipson 2010).

  13. 13.

    Contrary to Adler’s evocative article title, “The earliest musical tradition”, about the bird-bone flute discussed earlier.

  14. 14.

    Some theorists have interpreted particular overwrought, finely crafted Middle Palaeolithic stone handaxes as sexual signals—the ‘sexy handaxe’ hypothesis (see, e.g., Kohn and Mithen 1999)—though this is controversial (e.g., Nowell and Chang 2009). A possible interpretation of the mammoth-ivory flutes, similarly, sees these as ‘sexy flutes’. That possibility is compatible with the view argued here (that they reflect increasing social complexity and differentiation) and thus may be a part of the story. Ancient humans were presumably constantly scrutinizing one another for partnership making/unmaking; being a skilled craftsperson presumably would not go unnoticed by potential partners. That said, the view here does not explain them as products of sexual selection.

  15. 15.

    I’m grateful to Myriam Albor for helpful discussion about this point.

References

  • Adler D (2009) The earliest musical tradition. Nature 460:695–696

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Álvarez Fernández E, Jöris O (2007) Personal ornaments in the early Upper Paleolithic of western Eurasia: an evaluation of the record. Eurasian Prehist 5(2):31–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkins A (2013) Peirce’s theory of signs. In: Zalta EN (ed) The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (summer 2013 edition). http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2013/entries/peirce-semiotics

  • Barker G (2006) The agricultural revolution in prehistory: why did foragers become farmers? Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boehm C (2001) Hierarchy in the forest: the evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouzouggar A, Barton N, Vanhaeren M, d’Errico F, Collcutt S, Higham T, Hodge E, Parfitt S, Rhodes E, Schwenninger J-L, Stringer C, Turner E, Ward S, Moutmir A, Stambouli A (2007) 82,000-year-old shell beads from North Africa and implications for the origins of modern human behavior. PNAS 104(24):9964–9969

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brusse C (2019) Signaling theories of religion: models and explanation. Relig Brain Behav. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2019.1678514

  • Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD (1979) The roaring of red deer and the evolution of honest advertisemdent. Behaviour 69:145–170

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conard N, Malina M (2008) New evidence for the origins of music from the caves of the Swabian Jura. In: Both AA, Eichmann R, Hickmann E, Koch L-C (eds) Challenges and objectives in music archaeology. Studien zur Musikarchäologie VI, Orient-Archäologie 22. Verlag Marie Leidorf, Rahden, pp 13–22

    Google Scholar 

  • Conard N, Malina M, Münzel S, Seeberger F (2004) Eine Mammutelfenbeinflöte aus dem Aurignacien des Geißenklösterle: neue Belege für eine Musikalische Tradition im Frühen Jungpaläolithikum auf der Schwäbischen Alb. Archäologisches Korrespondenzblatt 34:447–462

    Google Scholar 

  • Conard N, Malina M, Münzel S (2009) New flutes document the earliest musical tradition in southwestern Germany. Nature 460:737–740

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Currie A (2018) Rock, bone and ruin: an optimist’s guide to the historical sciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Currie A, Killin A (2019) From things to thinking: cognitive archaeology. Mind Lang 34(2):263–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Lawson G, Vanhaeren M, Tillier A-M, Soressi M, Bresson F, Maureille B, Nowell A, Lakarra J, Backwell L, Julien M (2003) Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music—an interdisciplinary perspective. J World Prehist 17(1):1–70

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Vanhaeren M, van Niekerk K (2005) Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. J Hum Evol 48(1):3–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Errico F, Vanhaeren M, Barton N, Bouzouggar A, Mienis H, Richter D, Hublin J-J, McPherron SP, Lozouet P (2009) Additional evidence on the use of personal ornaments in the Middle Palaeolithic of North Africa. PNAS 106(38):16051–16056

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson KA, Kintsch W (1995) Long-term working memory. Psychol Rev 102(2):211–245

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flannery K, Marcus J (2012) The creation of inequality. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith P (2014a) Signs and symbolic behavior. Biol Theory 9:78–88

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith P (2014b) Sender-receiver systems within and between organisms. Philos Sci 81(5):866–878

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfrey-Smith P (2017) Senders, receivers, and symbolic artifacts. Biol Theory 12:275–286

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood C, d’Errico F, Yates R, Jacobs Z, Tribolo C, Duller GAT, Mercier N, Sealy J, Valladas H, Watts I, Wintle A (2002) Emergence of modern human behaviour: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science 295:1278–1280

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood C, d’Errico F, Vanhaeren M, van Niekerk K, Jacobs Z (2004) Middle Stone Age beads from South Africa. Science 304:404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood C, d’Errico F, Watts I (2009) Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. J Hum Evol 57:27–47

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan HS, Hooper PL, Gurven M (2009) The evolutionary and ecological roots of human social organization. Philos Trans R Soc B 364:3289–3299

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keller CM, Keller JD (1996) Cognition and tool use: the blacksmith at work. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (2014) Musicality in human evolution, archaeology and ethnography. Biol Philos 29:597–609

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (2016) Rethinking music’s status as adaptation versus technology: a niche construction perspective. Ethnomusicol Forum 25(2):210–233

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (2017) Plio-Pleistocene foundations of hominin musicality: coevolution of cognition, sociality, and music. Biol Theory 12(4):222–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killin A (2018) The origins of music: evidence, theory, and prospects. Music Sci. https://doi.org/10.1177/2059204317751971

  • Knauft B (1991) Violence and sociality in human evolution. Curr Anthropol 32:391–428

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohn M, Mithen S (1999) Handaxes: products of sexual selection. Antiquity 73:518–526

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn S (2014) Signaling theory and technologies of communication in the Paleolithic. Biol Theory 9(1):42–50

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn S, Stiner MC (1998) The earliest Aurignacian of Riparo Mochi (Liguria, Italy). Curr Anthropol 39(suppl-3):175–189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn S, Stiner MC (2007) Body ornamentation as information technology: towards an understanding of the significance of early beads. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O et al (eds) Rethinking the human revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins and dispersal of modern humans. MacDonald Institute of Archaeology, Cambridge, pp 45–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Leach E (1976) Culture and communication: the logic by which symbols are connected. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lévi-Strauss C (1969) The raw and the cooked. Harper & Row, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis D (1969) Convention. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA

    Google Scholar 

  • Lombard M, Haidle MN (2011) Thinking a bow-and-arrow set: cognitive implications of Middle Stone Age bow and stone-tipped arrow technology. Camb Archaeol J 22(2):237–264

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lombard M, Philllipson L (2010) Indications of bow and stone-tipped arrow use 64,000 years ago in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Antiquity 84:635–648

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty S, Brooks A (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behaviour. J Hum Evol 39:453–563

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty S, Stringer C (2007) Palaeoanthropology: the coast in colour. Nature 449:793–794

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morley I (2013) The prehistory of music: human evolution, archaeology and the origins of musicality. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nowell A, Chang ML (2009) The case against sexual selection as an explanation of handaxe morphology. PaleoAnthropology 2009:77–88

    Google Scholar 

  • Pain R (2019) What can the lithic record tell us about the evolution of hominin cognition? Topoi. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-019-09683-0

  • Saussure F (1959) Course in general linguistics. Philosophical Library, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth R, Cheney D, Marler P (1980) Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210:801–803

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheehan O, Watts J, Gray R, Atkinson Q (2018) Coevolution of landesque capital intensive agriculture and sociopolitical hierarchy. Proc Natl Acad Sci 115:3628–3633

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skyrms B (2004) Stag hunt and the evolution of social structure. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Skyrms B (2010) Signals. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny K (2012) The evolved apprentice: how evolution made humans unique. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stiner MC (2014) Finding a common bandwidth: causes of convergence and diversity in Paleolithic beads. Biol Theory 9:51–64

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Texier PJ, Porraz G, Parkington J et al (2010) A Howiesons Poort tradition of engraving ostrich egg shell containers dated to 60,000 years ago at Diepkloof Rock Shelter, South Africa. PNAS 107:6180–6185

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren M, d’Errico F, Stringer C, James SL, Todd JA, Mienis HK (2006) Middle Palaeolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science 312:1785–1788

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White R (2003) Prehistoric art: the symbolic journey of humankind. Harry N. Abrams, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiessner PW (2014) Embers of society: firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Proc Natl Acad Sci 111(39):14027–14035

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2004) The expert Neanderthal mind. J Hum Evol 46(4):467–487

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Anton Killin .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Killin, A. (2021). Music Archaeology, Signaling Theory, Social Differentiation. In: Killin, A., Allen-Hermanson, S. (eds) Explorations in Archaeology and Philosophy. Synthese Library, vol 433. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-61052-4_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics