Skip to main content

The Intersubjective Objectivity of Learnables

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Classroom-based Conversation Analytic Research

Part of the book series: Educational Linguistics ((EDUL,volume 46))

Abstract

This chapter delves into the theoretical underpinnings of praxeological and dialogical research on the emergence of opportunities for learning in teacher–student interactivities. First, I introduce the emergence of objects of learning as a social phenomenon; then I argue for the intersubjective–intercorporeal understanding of those objects as emergent learnables in classroom talk in their immediate contextual and interactional environments. Two sequences of classroom activities in a Swedish as a second language classroom are presented and analyzed from a phenomenological–sociological view on intersubjectivity. The analysis highlights the significance of a dialogical and praxeological approach to the study of learning/teaching activities, and underscores that attending to intersubjectivity includes paying attention to corporeal acts in the procedure of orienting to, and showing understanding about, learnables. The chapter concludes that, in order to understand teaching/learning behaviors, a detailed analysis of participants’ actions in their interactivities is necessary. More specifically, in all talk-in-interaction (and particularly in classroom talk, with which this study is specifically concerned), the objective reality of linguistic expressions – their forms, and their functions – is accomplished, situated and embodied, and is thus reflexive and indexical in nature. This may suggest that researchers abstain from the dichotomy of the subjective–objective reality of a learnable in favor of the possibility of considering the intersubjective objectivity of a learnable as what is accomplished in real time in a social activity.

I would like to thank the editors of the book, particularly Numa Markee for his detailed comments on this chapter and also for allowing me to use his own data to write the practical implications for the chapter. I am also indebted to Per Linell, Mathias Broth and Charlotta Plejert for their comments on the earlier version of the manuscript.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Durkheim in his seminal work Suicide (1897/1951, p. 37) states: “Sociological method as we practice it rests wholly on the basic principle that social facts must be studied as things, that is, as realities external to the individual.”

  2. 2.

    This falls within the discussion of representational modifications of objects in Husserl’s account of perception (Husserl 1983, pp. 90–91; cf. Duranti 2009, p. 206, who explains it as aspects of attention in the process of learning and socialization). Husserl (1983, p. 91) explains that the sense of anything that appears to us or presented in the world would rely on our standpoint and orientation to it because “[it] can ‘appear’ only in a certain ‘orientation’, which necessarily predelineates a system of possible new orientations each of which, in turn, corresponds to a certain ‘mode of appearance’ which we can express, say, givenness from such and such a ‘side’, and so forth.” (emphasis in original).

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1995). Learning and memory: An integrated approach. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrén, M. (2010). Children’s gestures from 18–30 months Doctoral dissertation, Lund University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anward, J. (2004). Lexeme recycled: How categories emerge from interaction. Logos and Language, V(2), 31–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. (1981). In M. Holquist (Ed.), The dialogic imagination. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. (1986). In C. Emerson & M. Holquist (Eds.), Speech, genre, and other late essays. Austin: University of Texas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (2011). Theory of language: The representational function of language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. (Original work published 1934).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). Extended mind. Analysis, 58(1), 7–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csordas, T. J. (2008). Intersubjectivity and intercorporeality. Subjectivity, 22, 110–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duranti, A. (2009). The relevance of Husserl’s theory to language socialization. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 19(2), 205–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, E. (1951). In G. Simpson (Ed.), Suicide. A study in sociology. Glencoe: The Free Press. (Original work published 1897).

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim, E. (1976). The elementary forms of the religious life (J. W. Swain, Trans.). New York/London: George Allen and Unwin. (Original work published 1915).

    Google Scholar 

  • Enfield, N. J., & Levinson, S. C. (2006). Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition and interaction. Oxford: Wenner–Gren Foundation for Anthropological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enfield, N. J., Kita, S., & de Ruiter, J. P. (2007). Primary and secondary pragmatic functions of pointing gestures. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 1722–1741.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eskildsen, S. W., & Markee, N. (2018). L2 talk as social accomplishment. AILA Applied Linguistics series, 17, 69–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Firth, A., & Wagner, J. (1997). On discourse, communication, and (some) fundamental concepts in SLA research. Modern Language Journal, 81(3), 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice–Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (2002). In A. W. Rawls (Ed.), Ethnomethodology’s program. Working out Durkheim’s aphorism. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H. (2007). Lebenswelt origins of the sciences: Working out Durkheim’s aphorism. Book two: Workplace and documentary diversity of ethnomethodological studies of work and sciences by ethnomethodology’s authors: What did we do? What did we learn? Human Studies, 30(3), 9–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfinkel, H., & Sacks, H. (1986). On formal structures of practical actions. In H. Garfinkel (Ed.), Ethnomethodological studies of work (pp. 160–193). London/New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1970).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in public places. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1967). Interactional ritual. Essays on face-to-face behavior. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. An essay on the organization of experience. Boston, Mass: Northeastern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2003). The body in action. In J. Coupland & R. Gwyn (Eds.), Discourse, the body and identity (pp. 19–42). New York: Palgrave/Macmillan.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2007). Environmentally coupled gestures. In S. D. Duncan, J. Cassell, & E. T. Levy (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language (pp. 195–212). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2013). The co-operative, transformative organization of human action and knowledge. Journal of Pragmatics, 46, 8–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C. (2017). Co-operative action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C., & Duranti, A. (1992). Rethinking context: An introduction. In A. Duranti & C. Goodwin (Eds.), Rethinking context. Language as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 1–42). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, C., & Salomon, R. (2019). Not being bound by what you can see now. Charles Goodwin in conversation with René Salomon [32 paragraphs]. Forum Qualitative Sozialforschung/Forum: Qualitative Social Research, 20(2), Art. 11. https://doi.org/10.17169/fqs-20.2.3271.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, R. (2010). Psychology. The science of mind and behavior (6th ed.). Abington: Hodder Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanks, W. (1990). Language and lived space among the Maya. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1967). What is a thing? (W. B. Barton, Jr. & V. Deutsch, Trans.). South Bend: Gateway Editions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. (1984). A change-of-state token and aspects of its sequential placement. In M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structure of social action (pp. 229–345). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. (1987). Ethnomethodology. In A. Giddens & J. Turner (Eds.), Social theory today (pp. 224–272). Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hindmarsh, J., Reynolds, P., & Dunne, S. (2011). Exhibiting understanding: The body in apprenticeship. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 489–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1969). Formal and transcendental logic (D. Cairns, Trans.). The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff. (Original work published 1929).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1983). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. First book: General introduction to a pure phenomenology (F. Kersten, Trans.). Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1989). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. In Second book: Studies on the phenomenology of constitution (R. Rojcevicz & A. Schuwer, Trans.). Kluwer Academic Publishers. (Original work published 1952).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (2001). Logical investigation (Vol. 2). New York: Routledge. (Original work 1900–1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingold, T. (2007). Lines. A brief history. Oxon/New York: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G. (1979). A technique for inviting laughter and its subsequent acceptance declination. In G. Psathas (Ed.), Everyday language: Studies in ethnomethodology (pp. 79–96). New York: Irvington Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G. (1987). On exposed and embedded correction in conversation. In G. Button & J. R. E. Lee (Eds.), Talk and social organization (pp. 86–100). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kääntä, L. (this volume). Multimodal perspective into teachers’ definitional practices: Comparing subject-specific language in physics and history lessons. In S. Kunitz, N. Markee, & O. Sert (Eds.), Classroom-based conversation analytic research: Theoretical and applied perspectives on pedagogy. Cham: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koschmann, T., LeBaron, C., Goodwin, C., Zemel, A., & Dunnington, G. (2007). Formulating the triangle of doom. Gesture, 7(1), 97–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koschmann, T., LeBaron, C., Goodwin, C., & Feltovich, P. (2011). ‘Can you see the cystic artery yet?’ A simple matter of trust. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 521–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. (1992). Activity types and language. In P. Drew & J. Heritage (Eds.), Talk at work (pp. 66–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. (2013). Action formation and ascription. In J. Sidnell & T. Stivers (Eds.), The handbook of conversation analysis (pp. 103–130). Malden/Oxford: Wiley–Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindwall, O., & Lymer, G. (2008). The dark matter of lab work: Illuminating the negotiation of disciplined perception in mechanics. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 17(2), 180–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Linell, P. (2009). Rethinking language, mind, and world dialogically. Interactional and contextual theories of human sense-making. Charlotte: Information Age Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linell, P. (2014). Interactivities, intersubjectivities, and language: On dialogism and phenomenology. Language and Dialogue, 4(2), 165–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macbeth, D. (1996). The discovery of situated worlds: Analytic commitments, or moral orders? Human Studies, 19, 267–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macbeth, D. (2000). Classrooms as installations. Direct instruction in the early grades. In S. Hester & D. Francis (Eds.), Local educational order. Ethnomethodological studies of knowledge in action. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L., & Renfrew, C. (2010). The cognitive life of things: Recasting the boundaries of the mind. Cambridge: McDonald institute for archeological research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markee, N. (1994). Toward and ethnomethodological respecification of second-language acquisition studies. In E. E. Tarone, S. M. Gass, & A. D. Cohen (Eds.), Research methodology in second-language acquisition (pp. 89–116). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markee, N. (2007). Conversation analysis for second language acquisition. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (pp. 355–374). New York/London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markee, N. (2008). Toward a learning behavior tracking methodology for CA-for-SLA. Applied Lingusitics, 29, 404–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau–Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London/New York: Routledge. (Original work published 1945).

    Google Scholar 

  • Mondada, L., & Pekarek Doehler, S. (2004). Second language acquisition as situated practice: Task accomplishment in the French second language classroom. The Modern Language Journal, 88, 501–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pike, K. L. (1954). Language in relation to a unified theory of the structure of human behavior. Glendale: Summer Institutes of Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. (1984a). Notes on methodology. In M. J. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 21–27). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. (1984b). On doing ‘being ordinary. In M. J. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 413–429). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on conversation (Vol. 1 & 2). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50(4), 696–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1967). The phenomenology of the social world. Chicago: Northwestern University Press. (Original work published 1932).

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1973). Collected papers I. the problem of social reality. The Hage: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1975). In I. Schutz (Ed.), Collected papers III. Studies in phenomenological philosophy. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J., & Jordan, J. S. (2009). Communication as a dynamic self-sustained system: The importance of time-scales and nested context. Communication Theory, 19, 445–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice. Learning, meaning and identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein. (2009). Philosophical investigations (G. E. M. Anscombe, P. M. S. Hacker, & J. Schulte, Trans.). Malden/Oxford: Wiley–Blackwell. (Original work published, 1953).

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ali Reza Majlesi .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Conventions for Transcribing Embodied Conduct

Conventions for Transcribing Embodied Conduct

#

hashtag: the position of an image within a turn at talk

 

dotted square bracket: aligning the position of an image with its nonverbal description and a turn at talk

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Majlesi, A.R. (2021). The Intersubjective Objectivity of Learnables. In: Kunitz, S., Markee, N., Sert, O. (eds) Classroom-based Conversation Analytic Research. Educational Linguistics, vol 46. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-52193-6_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-52193-6_4

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-52192-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-52193-6

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics