Abstract
Mentalizing, or theory of mind, is the ability to infer the mental states, such as desires and beliefs, that motivate others’ actions. Mentalizing is fundamental to human social life, underpinning many of the forms of communication, cooperation, and culture that are likely unique to our species. But how did human mentalizing evolve? Comparative experiments with humans and other primates suggest that theory of mind has deep phylogenetic roots: its most basic foundations appear to be shared widely among primates while more sophisticated precursors are found only in our closer relatives, suggesting that they may have been present only in more recent common ancestors. Of course, still other features of human theory of mind have evolved uniquely in the human lineage, in the past 6–9 million years since we diverged from the other apes. Inter- and intra-specific studies suggest that mentalizing has likely evolved in response to the demands of competing and cooperating with others, with dietary and metabolic adaptations probably playing an important role as well. Future comparative work will continue to prove fruitful in determining exactly what it is about human mentalizing that makes us human and how it came to be.
Keywords
- Social cognition
- Theory of mind
- Mind-reading
- Perspective-taking
- Cognitive evolution
- Human evolution
- Primate
- Nonhuman
- Monkey
- Ape
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Buying options

References
Adolphs, R. (2009). The social brain: Neural basis of social knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 693–716. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163514
Aiello, L. C., & Wheeler, P. (1995). The expensive-tissue hypothesis: The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Current Anthropology, 36(2), 199–221.
Anderson, J. R., Kuroshima, H., Takimoto, A., & Fujita, K. (2013). Third-party social evaluation of humans by monkeys. Nature Communications, 4, 1561. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms2495
Ashton, B. J., Ridley, A. R., Edwards, E. K., & Thornton, A. (2018). Cognitive performance is linked to group size and affects fitness in Australian magpies. Nature, 554(7692), 364–367. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25503
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R. M., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(3), 110–118.
Banaji, M. R., & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.). (2012). Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Bettle, R., & Rosati, A. G. (2019). Flexible gaze-following in rhesus monkeys. Animal Cognition, 22, 673. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-019-01263-4
Bond, A. B., Kamil, A. C., & Balda, R. P. (2003). Social complexity and transitive inference in corvids. Animal Behaviour, 65, 479–487.
Boyd, R. (2006). The puzzle of human sociality. Science, 314, 1555–1556.
Brauer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119(2), 145–154. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.145
Brauer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees really know what others can see in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 10(4), 439–448.
Brauer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees do not take into account what others can hear in a competitive situation. Animal Cognition, 11(1), 175–178. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0097-0
Bray, J., Krupenye, C., & Hare, B. (2014). Ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) exploit information about what others can see but not what they can hear. Animal Cognition, 17(3), 735–744. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0705-0
Bullinger, A. F., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, prefer individual over collaborative strategies towards goals. Animal Behaviour, 82(5), 1135–1141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.08.008
Burkart, J., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & van Schaik, C. (2012). Even simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 129–138. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026025
Burkart, J. M., & Heschl, A. (2007). Understanding visual access in common marmosets, Callithrix jacchus: Perspective taking or behaviour reading? Animal Behaviour, 73, 457–469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.05.019
Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Great apes distinguish true from false beliefs in an interactive helping task. PLoS One, 12(4), e0173793.
Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally. Developmental Science, 10(4), F31–F38. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00630.x
Buttelmann, D., Schütte, S., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Great apes infer others’ goals based on context. Animal Cognition, 15, 1037–1053. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0528-4
Butterfill, S. A., & Apperly, I. A. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind & Language, 28(5), 606–637.
Byrne, R. W., & Bates, L. A. (2007). Sociality, evolution and cognition. Current Biology, 17(16), R714–R723.
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. W. (1988). Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7(4), 488–498.
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112(2), 192–206. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.192
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes. Child Development, 70(2), 381–395.
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 187–192.
Canteloup, C., & Meunier, H. (2017). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Tonkean macaques’ understanding of human goal-directed actions. PeerJ, 5, e3227. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.3227
Canteloup, C., Piraux, E., Poulin, N., & Meunier, H. (2016). Do Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) perceive what conspecifics do and do not see? PeerJ, 4, e1693. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.1693
Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Harvey, P. H. (1980). Primates, brains and ecology. Journal of Zoology, 190, 309–323.
Corballis, M. C. (2011). The recursive mind: The origins of human language, thought, and civilization. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22(2), 142–146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (2007). Baboons eavesdrop to deduce mating opportunities. Animal Behaviour, 73(5), 885–890. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.016
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: John Murray.
de Waal, F. B. (1982). Chimpanzee politics: Power and sex among apes. New York, NY: Harper and Row.
DeCasien, A. R., Williams, S. A., & Higham, J. P. (2017). Primate brain size is predicted by diet but not sociality. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 1, 0112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-017-0112
Defolie, C., Malassis, R., Serre, M., & Meunier, H. (2015). Tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) adapt their communicative behaviour to human’s attentional states. Animal Cognition, 18(3), 747–755. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0841-9
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2014). Capuchins’ (Cebus apella) sensitivity to others’ goal-directed actions in a helping context. Animal Cognition, 17(3), 689–700.
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2017). Do rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta, understand what others know when gaze following? Animal Behaviour, 134, 193–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.016
Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2018). What do monkeys know about others’ knowledge? Cognition, 170, 201–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.004
Dunbar, R. I., & Shultz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317(5843), 1344–1347.
Emery, N. J., & Clayton, N. S. (2004). The mentality of crows: Convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science, 306(5703), 1903–1907. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1098410
Flavel, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., & Flavel, E. R. (1981). Young children’s knowledge about visual perception: Further evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 distinction. Developmental Psychology, 17(1), 99–103.
Flombaum, J. I., & Santos, L. R. (2005). Rhesus monkeys attribute perceptions to others. Current Biology, 15(5), 447–452.
Hare, B., Addessi, E., Call, J., Tomasello, M., & Visalberghi, E. (2003). Do capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella, know what conspecifics do and do not see? Animal Behaviour, 65, 131–142.
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771–785.
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61(1), 139–151.
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees deceive a human competitor by hiding. Cognition, 101(3), 495–514.
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernandez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317(5843), 1360–1366.
Heyes, C. (1998). Theory of mind in nonhuman primates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21(1), 101–114. Discussion 115–148.
Hopkins, W. D., Russell, J. L., & Schaeffer, J. (2014). Chimpanzee intelligence is heritable. Current Biology, 24(14), 1649–1652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.076
Horschler, D. J., Santos, L. R., & MacLean, E. L. (2019). Do non-human primates really represent others’ ignorance? A test of the awareness relations hypothesis. Cognition, 190, 72–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.04.012
Horton, K. E., & Caldwell, C. A. (2006). Visual co-orientation and expectations about attentional orientation in pileated gibbons (Hylobates pileatus). Behavioural Processes, 72(1), 65–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.12.004
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P. Bateson & R. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur social behavior and primate intelligence. Science, 153, 501–506.
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know, but not what they believe. Cognition, 109(2), 224–234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.010
Kano, F., & Call, J. (2014). Great apes generate goal-based action predictions: An eye-tracking study. Psychological Science, 25(9), 1691–1698. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614536402
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., & Call, J. (2017). Eye tracking uncovered great apes’ ability to anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10(2), e1299836. https://doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2017.1299836
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Submentalizing cannot explain belief-based action anticipation in apes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 633–634. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.06.011
Kano, F., Krupenye, C., Hirata, S., Tomonaga, M., & Call, J. (2019). Great apes use self-experience to anticipate an agent’s action in a false belief test. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 116, 20904.
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015a). Chimpanzees strategically manipulate what others can see. Animal Cognition, 18(5), 1069–1076. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0875-z
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015b). The goggles experiment: Can chimpanzees use self-experience to infer what a competitor can see? Animal Behaviour, 105, 211–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.04.028
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Differing views: Can chimpanzees do Level 2 perspective-taking? Animal Cognition, 19(3), 555–564. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-016-0956-7
Kovacs, A. M., Teglas, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330(6012), 1830–1834. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1190792
Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). A competitive nonverbal false belief task for children and apes. Developmental Science, 12(4), 521–535. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00793.x
Krachun, C., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2010). A new change-of-contents false belief test: Children and chimpanzees compared. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 23, 145–165.
Krupenye, C., & Call, J. (2019). Theory of mind in animals: Current and future directions. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 10, e1503. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1503
Krupenye, C., & Hare, B. (2018). Bonobos prefer individuals that hinder others over those that help. Current Biology, 28(2), 280–286.e285. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.11.061
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110–114.
Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes’ performance in a false belief task inanimate control. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10(4), e1343771. https://doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771
Krupenye, C., MacLean, E., & Hare, B. (2017). Does the bonobo have a (chimpanzee-like) theory of mind? In B. Hare & S. Yamamoto (Eds.), Bonobos: Unique in mind, brain and behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Krupenye, C., Tan, J., & Hare, B. (2018). Bonobos voluntarily hand food to others but not toys or tools. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 285(1886), 20181536. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.1536
Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & Burkart, J. M. (2013). Do robots have goals? How agent cues influence action understanding in non-human primates. Behavioural Brain Research, 246, 47–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.01.047
Liebal, K., & Kaminski, J. (2012). Gibbons (Hylobates pileatus, H. moloch, H. lar, Symphalangus syndactylus) follow human gaze, but do not take the visual perspective of others. Animal Cognition, 15, 1211–1216. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0543-5
Lorincz, E. N., Jellema, T., Gómez, J. C., Barraclough, N., Xiao, D., & Perrett, D. I. (2005). Do monkeys understand actions and minds of others? Studies of single cells and eye movements. In S. Dehaene, J. R. Duhamel, M. D. Hauser, & G. Rizzolatti (Eds.), From monkey brain to human brain: A Fyssen Foundation Symposium (pp. 189–210). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lurz, R. (2009). If chimpanzees are mindreaders, could behavioral science tell? Toward a solution of the logical problem. Philosophical Psychology, 22(3), 305–328. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515080902970673
Lurz, R., Krachun, C., Mahovetz, L., Wilson, M. J. G., & Hopkins, W. (2018). Chimpanzees gesture to humans in mirrors: Using reflection to dissociate seeing from line of gaze. Animal Behaviour, 135, 239–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.11.014
MacLean, E. L., & Hare, B. (2012). Bonobos and chimpanzees infer the target of another’s attention. Animal Behaviour, 83(2), 345–353. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.10.026
MacLean, E. L., & Hare, B. (2013). Spontaneous triadic engagement in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 127(3), 245–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030935
MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R. C., … Zhao, Y. (2014). The evolution of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(20), E2140–E2148.
MacLean, E. L., Matthews, L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Anderson, R. C., Aureli, F., … Wobber, V. (2012). How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. Animal Cognition, 15, 223–238.
MacLean, E. L., Merritt, D. J., & Brannon, E. M. (2008). Social complexity predicts transitive reasoning in prosimian primates. Animal Behaviour, 76(2), 479–486. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.025
MacLean, E. L., Sandel, A. A., Bray, J., Oldenkamp, R. E., Reddy, R. B., & Hare, B. A. (2013). Group size predicts social but not nonsocial cognition in lemurs. PLoS One, 8(6), e66359. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066359
MacLean, E. L., Sandel, A. A., Reddy, R., Bray, J., Oldenkamp, R., & Hare, B. (2013). Group size predicts social, but not nonsocial cognition in lemurs. PLoS One, 8(6), e66359. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066359
Marticorena, D. C. W., Ruiz, A. M., Mukerji, C., Goddu, A., & Santos, L. R. (2011). Monkeys represent others’ knowledge but not their beliefs. Developmental Science, 14(6), 1406–1416. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01085.x
Martin, A., & Santos, L. R. (2014). The origins of belief representation: Monkeys fail to automatically represent others’ beliefs. Cognition, 130(3), 300–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.016
Martin, A., & Santos, L. R. (2016). What cognitive representations support primate theory of mind? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(5), 375–382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.005
Melis, A. P., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) conceal visual and auditory information from others. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120(2), 154–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.2.154
Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) strategic helping in a collaborative task. Biology Letters, 9(2), 20130009. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2013.0009
Meltzoff, A. N., & Brooks, R. (2008). Self-experience as a mechanism for learning about others: A training study in social cognition. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1257–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012888
Meunier, H. (2017). Do monkeys have a theory of mind? How to answer the question? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 82, 110–123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.007
Milton, K. (1988). Foraging behavior and the evolution of primate cognition. In A. Whiten & R. Byrne (Eds.), Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in monkeys, apes and humans (pp. 285–305). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months of age. Child Development, 82(2), 661–673. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01571.x
Muller, M. N., Wrangham, R. W., & Pilbeam, D. R. (2017). Chimpanzees and human evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2000). Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114(4), 381–391.
Nunn, C. L. (2011). The comparative approach in evolutionary anthropology and biology. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
O’Grady, C., Kliesch, C., Smith, K., & Scott-Phillips, T. C. (2015). The ease and extent of recursive mindreading, across implicit and explicit tasks. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(4), 313–322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.01.004
Okamoto-Barth, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Great apes’ understanding of other individuals’ line of sight. Psychological Science, 18(5), 462–468.
Onishi, K. H., & Bailargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258.
Penn, D., & Povinelli, D. (2007). On the lack of evidence that non-human animals possess anything remotely resembling a ‘theory of mind’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 731–744. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2023. J577253MH830J082 [pii].
Phillips, W., Barnes, J. L., Mahajan, N., Yamaguchi, M., & Santos, L. R. (2009). ‘Unwilling’ versus ‘unable’: Capuchin monkeys’ (Cebus apella) understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 12(6), 938–945. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1467-7687.2009.00840.X
Pontzer, H., Brown, M. H., Raichlen, D. A., Dunsworth, H., Hare, B., Walker, K., … Ross, S. R. (2016). Metabolic acceleration and the evolution of human brain size and life history. Nature, 533, 390–392. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature17654
Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). Chimpanzees: Join visual attention. Psychological Science, 7(3), 129–135.
Povinelli, D. J., & Vonk, J. (2003). Chimpanzee minds: Suspiciously human? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(4), 157–160. S1364661303000536 [pii].
Powell, L. E., Isler, K., & Barton, R. A. (2017). Re-evaluating the link between brain size and behavioural ecology in primates. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 284(1865), 20171765. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1765
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.
Rekers, Y., Haun, D., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to collaborate. Current Biology, 21, 1756.
Rochat, M. J., Serra, E., Fadiga, L., & Gallese, V. (2008). The evolution of social cognition: Goal familiarity shapes monkeys’ action understanding. Current Biology, 18(3), 227–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.12.021
Rosati, A. G. (2017). Foraging cognition: Reviving the ecological intelligence hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(9), 691–702. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.05.011
Rosati, A. G., & Hare, B. (2009). Looking past the model species: Diversity in gaze-following skills across primates. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19(1), 45–51.
Ruiz, A., Gomez, J. C., Roeder, J. J., & Byrne, R. W. (2009). Gaze following and gaze priming in lemurs. Animal Cognition, 12, 427.
Sandel, A. A., MacLean, E., & Hare, B. (2011). Evidence from four lemur species that ringtailed lemur social cognition converges with that of haplorhine primates. Animal Behaviour, 81, 925–931.
Santos, L. R., Nissen, A. G., & Ferrugia, J. A. (2006). Rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta, know what others can and cannot hear. Animal Behaviour, 71(5), 1175–1181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.007
Senju, A., Southgate, V., Snape, C., Leonard, M., & Csibra, G. (2011). Do 18-month-olds really attribute mental states to others? A critical test. Psychological Science, 22(7), 878–880. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611411584
Shepherd, S. V., & Platt, M. L. (2008). Spontaneous social orienting and gaze following in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta). Animal Cognition, 11(1), 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-007-0083-6
Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587–592. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
Steiper, M. E., & Young, N. M. (2006). Primate molecular divergence dates. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution, 41(2), 384–394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ympev.2006.05.021
Sterelny, K. (2019). The origins of multi-level society. Topoi. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-019-09666-1
Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.
Tomasello, M. (2018). How children come to understand false beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(34), 8491–8498. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1804761115
Tomasello, M., Call, J., & Hare, B. (1998). Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 55, 1063–1069.
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70(279), vii.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675.
Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12-and 18-month-olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 906–912.
Tomasello, M., Hare, B., Lehmann, H., & Call, J. (2007). Reliance on head versus eyes in the gaze following of great apes and human infants: The cooperative eye hypothesis. Journal of Human Evolution, 52(3), 314–320.
Tomasello, M., Melis, A. P., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation. Current Anthropology, 53(6), 673–692. https://doi.org/10.1086/668207
Tremblay, S., Sharika, K. M., & Platt, M. L. (2017). Social decision-making and the brain: A comparative perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(4), 265–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.007
Warneken, F., Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biology, 5(7), e184.
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 1301–1303. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1121448
Whiten, A. (2013). Humans are not alone in computing how others see the world. Animal Behaviour, 86(2), 213–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.04.021
Whiten, A., & Byrne, R. W. (1988). Tactical deception in primates. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11(02), 233–273. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00049682
Wittig, R. M., Crockford, C., Langergraber, K. E., & Zuberbuhler, K. (2014). Triadic social interactions operate across time: A field experiment with wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Biological Sciences, 281(1779), 20133155. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3155
Wobber, V., Hare, B., Maboto, J., Lipson, S., Wrangham, R., & Ellison, P. T. (2010). Differential changes in steroid hormones before competition in bonobos and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(28), 12457–12462.
Wrangham, R. W. (2009). Catching fire: How cooking made us human. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M. (2012). Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(9), 3588–3592.
Acknowledgements
I am thankful to Josep Call, Brian Hare, Richard Byrne, and Andrew Whiten for discussions over the years on topics relevant to this paper. I am also grateful to the European Commission for financial support (Marie Skłodowska-Curie fellowship MENTALIZINGORIGINS). Finally, abundant thanks are owed to Michael Gilead and Kevin Ochsner, for their kind invitation to participate in this exciting compilation and for their enduring patience and support.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Krupenye, C. (2021). The Evolution of Mentalizing in Humans and Other Primates. In: Gilead, M., Ochsner, K.N. (eds) The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51890-5_6
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51890-5_6
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Cham
Print ISBN: 978-3-030-51889-9
Online ISBN: 978-3-030-51890-5
eBook Packages: Behavioral Science and PsychologyBehavioral Science and Psychology (R0)