Skip to main content

The Role of Fluency and Dysfluency in Metacognitive Experiences

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Trends and Prospects in Metacognition Research across the Life Span

Abstract

Metacognition is a broad term that means different things to researchers in different sub-areas. A major contribution of Anastastia Efklides is to bring together disparate approaches in metacognition under one theoretical perspective. In this paper, we examine the concept of fluency and how it has been employed in metacognition research. Fluency-based judgments are generally considered to be the primary source of inaccuracy of metacognitive judgments as well as the primary reason why metacognitive control goes astray in self-regulated learning. We discuss how and when fluent processing influences metacognition, when fluency leads to accurate judgments and when it leads to illusions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13, 219–235. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309341564.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arango-Muñoz, S. (2019). Cognitive phenomenology and metacognitive feelings. Mind and Language, 34, 247–262. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ball, B. H., Klein, K. N., & Brewer, G. A. (2014). Processing fluency mediates the influence of perceptual information on monitoring learning of educationally relevant materials. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20, 336–348.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Begg, I., Duft, S., Lalonde, P., Melnick, R., & Sanvito, J. (1989). Memory predictions are based on ease of processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 610–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Begg, I. M., Anas, A., & Farinacci, S. (1992). Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 446–458. https://doi.org/10.1037//0096-3445.121.4.446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, A. S., Bjork, R. A., & Schwartz, B. L. (1998). The mismeasure of memory: When retrieval fluency is misleading as a metamnemonic index. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 55–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.127.1.55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Besken, M. (2018). Generating lies produces lower memory predictions and higher memory performance than telling the truth: Evidence for a metacognitive illusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44, 465–484.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Besken, M., & Mulligan, N. W. (2014). Perceptual fluency, auditory generation, and metamemory: Analyzing the perceptual fluency hypothesis in the auditory modality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 429–440. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034407.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., & Bjork, E. L. (1992). A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation. In A. F. Healy, S. M. Kosslyn, & R. M. Shiffrin (Eds.), From learning processes to cognitive processes: Essays in honor of William K. Estes (Vol. 2, pp. 35–67). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 417–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, A. B., & Castel, A. D. (2018). On belief and fluency in the construction of judgments of learning: Assessing and altering the direct effects of belief. Acta Psychologica, 186, 27–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A. (2008). Metacognition: Defining its facets and levels of functioning in relation to self-and co-regulation. European Psychologist, 13, 277–287. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.13.4.277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A. (2011). Interactions of metacognition with motivation and affect in self-regulated learning: The MASRL model. Educational Psychologist, 46, 6–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2011.538645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A. (2016). Metamemory and affect. In J. Dunlosky & S. Tauber (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 245–268). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A. (2018). Gifted students and self-regulated learning: The MASRL model and its implications for SRL. High Abilities Studies, 30, 79–102. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598139.2018.1556069.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A., Schwartz, B. L., & Brown, V. (2018). Motivation and affect in self-regulated learning: Does metacognition play a role? In D. H. Schunk & J. A. Greene (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance (pp. 64–82). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new era of cognitive- developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldinger, S. D., & Hansen, W. A. (2005). Remembering by the seat of your pants. Psychological Science, 16, 525–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., Goldstein, D., & Toppino, T. (1977). Frequency and the conference of referential validity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16, 107–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jemstedt, A., Schwartz, B. L., & Jönsson, F. U. (2018). Ease-of-learning judgments are based on both processing fluency and beliefs. Memory, 26, 807–815. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2017.1410849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kleider, H. M., & Goldinger, S. D. (2004). Illusions of face memory: Clarity breeds familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 196–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1993). How do we know that we know? The accessibility account of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review, 100, 609–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Ma’ayan, H. (2005). The effects of encoding fluency and retrieval fluency on judgments of learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 478–492. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., Rhodes, M. G., Castel, A. D., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The ease-of-processing heuristic and the stability bias: Dissociating memory, memory beliefs, and memory judgments. Psychological Science, 22, 787–794. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611407929.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, K., Albuquerque, P. B., & Martín-Luengo, B. (2018). Cognitive load eliminates the effect of perceptual information on judgments of learning with sentences. Memory & Cognition. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0853-1.

  • Metcalfe, J. (1993). Novelty monitoring, metacognition, and control in a composite holographic associative recall model: Interpretations for Korsakoff amnesia. Psychological Review, 100, 3–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2008). Familiarity and retrieval processes in delayed judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1084–1097. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012580.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Son, L. K. (2012). Anoetic, noetic, and autonoetic metacognition. In M. Beran, J. R. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust (Eds.), The foundations of metacognition (pp. 289–301). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., Schwartz, B. L., & Joaquim, S. G. (1993). The cue-familiarity heuristic in metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 851–861. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.19.4.851.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, M., & Dunlosky, J. (2017). How beliefs can impact judgments of learning: Evaluating analytic processing theory with beliefs about fluency. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 245–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, M. L., Tauber, S. K., & Dunlosky, J. (2013). Contributions of beliefs and processing fluency to the effect of relatedness on judgments of learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 378–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, M. L., Dunlosky, J., Tauber, S. K., & Rhodes, M. (2014). The font size effect on judgments of learning: Does it exemplify fluency effects or reflect people’s beliefs about memory? Journal of Memory and Language, 70, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 125–173). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004). Spontaneous discounting of availability in frequency judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15, 100–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oppenheimer, D. M. (2008). The secret life of fluency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 237–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Proust, J. (2015). The representational structure of feelings. In T. Metzinger & J. M. Windt (Eds.), Open MIND (Vol. 31). Frankfurt am Main: Open MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570047.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., & Greifeneder, R. (2017). Processing fluency in education: How metacognitive feelings shape learning, belief formation, and affect. Educational Psychologist, 52, 84–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truth. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 338–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reder, L. M. (1987). Strategy selection in question answering. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 90–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G. (2016). Judgments of learning: Methods, data, and theory. In J. Dunlosky & S. Tauber (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 65–80). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Castel, A. D. (2008). Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 615–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Tauber, S. K. (2011). The influence of delaying judgements of learning (JOLs) on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 131–148. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., & Cleary, A. M. (2016). Tip-of-the-tongue states, déjà vu and other metacognitive oddities. In J. Dunlosky & S. Tauber (Eds.), Oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 95–108). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., & Metcalfe, J. (2011). Tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states: Retrieval, behavior, and experience. Memory & Cognition, 39, 737–749.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, B. L., & Metcalfe, J. (2014). Tip-of-the-tongue (TOT) states: Mechanisms and metacognitive control. In B. L. Schwartz & A. S. Brown (Eds.), Tip-of-the-tongue states and related phenomena (pp. 15–31). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N. (2018). Of fluency, beauty, and truth: Inferences from metacognitive experiences. In J. Proust & M. Fortier (Eds.), Metacognitive diversity (pp. 25–46). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simmons, J. P., & Nelson, L. D. (2006). Intuitive confidence: Choosing between intuitive and nonintuitive alternatives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 409–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, H., & Schwarz, N. (2008). Fluency and the detection of misleading questions: Low processing fluency attenuates the Moses illusion. Social Cognition, 26, 791–799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, A. K., Lee, M., & Hughes, G. (2016). Introspecting on the elusive: The uncanny state of the feeling of knowing. In J. Dunlosky & S. Tauber (Eds.), Oxford handbook of metamemory (pp. 81–94). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toftness, A. R., Carpenter, S. K., Geller, J., Lauber, S., Johnson, M., & Armstrong, P. (2018). Instructor fluency leads to higher confidence in learning, but not better learning. Metacognition and Learning, 13, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E., & Pearlstone, Z. (1966). Availability versus accessibility of information in memory for words. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 381–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Undorf, M., & Zimdahl, M. F. (2018). Metamemory and memory for a wide range of font sizes: What is the contribution of perceptual fluency? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 45, 97–109. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000571.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Undorf, M., Zimdahl, M. F., & Bernstein, D. M. (2017). Perceptual fluency contributes to effects of stimulus size on judgments of learning. Journal of Memory and Language, 92, 293–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2016.07.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Undorf, M., Söllner, A., & Bröder, A. (2018). Simultaneous utilization of multiple cues in judgments of learning. Memory & Cognition, 46, 507–519. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-017-0780-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Unkelbach, C. (2006). The learned interpretation of cognitive fluency. Psychological Science, 17, 339–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verschuere, B., Spruyt, A., Meijer, E. H., & Otgaar, H. (2011). The ease of lying. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 908–911. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weissgerber, S. C., & Reinhard, M.-A. (2017). Is disfluency desirable for learning? Learning and Instruction, 49, 199–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wells, A., & Colbear, J. S. (2012). Treating posttraumatic stress disorder with metacognitive therapy: A preliminary controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 373–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, C., Huang, T. S.-T., & Shanks, D. R. (2018). Perceptual fluency affects judgments of learning: The font size effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 99, 99–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bennett L. Schwartz .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Schwartz, B.L., Jemstedt, A. (2021). The Role of Fluency and Dysfluency in Metacognitive Experiences. In: Moraitou, D., Metallidou, P. (eds) Trends and Prospects in Metacognition Research across the Life Span. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-51673-4_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics