Skip to main content

Making Sense of Syntactic Error in Conversations Between Persons with Dementia and Their Non-impaired Co-participants

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Learning from the Talk of Persons with Dementia

Part of the book series: The Language of Mental Health ((TLMH))

Abstract

In this chapter, the authors demonstrate how syntactic analysis combines with interactional methods to form a powerful tool in understanding the language produced by persons with dementia. They discuss the relationship between syntactic structure and interactional behavior in conversations. Specifically, they present two cases of persons with dementia exhibiting great syntactic impairment symptomatic of late-stage dementia. To help caregivers, practitioners, and novice researchers gain a better understanding of the linguistic effects of both dementia and co-participant responses, they examine incidences of speaker error in which co-participants respond to impaired syntax as making sense and the participants with dementia are able to employ better communicative language, if only briefly. From these observations, the authors posit that co-participants’ linguistic and interactional choices may allow syntactic and interactional competency to emerge during interactional collaborations and, as such, serve as possible models of (best) communication practices applicable to home, clinic, and research settings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    We chose the LGSWE benchmark corpus to be representative of the syntactic patterns reflective of non-impaired conversational participants as it contains over 3.9 million words in the British English conversation sub-corpus and approximately 2.5 million words in the American English conversation sub-corpus. We compared the LGSWE conversation data to our corpus of 11,482 words obtained from 20 conversations between persons with dementia and their non-impaired co-participants.

  2. 2.

    The most frequently occurring ten verbs in the LGSWE conversation corpus, presented here in descending frequency, are (1) say, (2) get, (3) go, (4) know, (5) think, (6) see, (7) make, (8) come, (9) take, and (10) want (Biber et al., 1999, p. 375). The most frequently occurring ten verbs in the talk of persons with dementia in our corpus, in descending frequency, are (1) have, (2) know, (3) get, (4) come, (5) go, (6) think, (7) do, (8) take, (9) want, (10) make/give (Stickle & Wanner, 2017, p. 53). Verbs shared by each are emboldened.

  3. 3.

    Reports that persons with dementia have difficulty with closed class grammatical items are few. Alegria et al. (2013) showed that preposition use in the talk of persons in moderate stages of Alzheimer’s disease were lower than non-impaired persons, but this study concerns comparative usage, not selection error. Meteyard and Patterson (2009) discuss the debate on effects of dementia on closed class retrieval while reporting evidence in their data indicating possible difficulties in preposition selection in the talk of persons diagnosed with semantic dementia.

  4. 4.

    Also referred to as recurrent perseveration, e.g., Bayles, Tomoeda, Kaszniak, Stern, and Eagans (1985).

  5. 5.

    This area is composed of premotor, posterior parietal and frontal-parietal opercula cortices, right inferior frontal, superior temporal cortices, and basal ganglia.

  6. 6.

    This evidence supports the view that syntactic evidence is housed in the lemma stratum (Pickering & Branigan, 1998; Roelofs, 1992) and the psycholinguistic model positing that language production relies on a three stage process: (1) a prelinguistic phase, (2) a phase of linguistic formulation that is aligned with and fitted to the intended interlocutor prior to linguistic expression, and (3) linguistic expression (Dell, 1986; Levelt, 1989; Levelt, Roelofs, & Meyer, 1999).

References

  • Alegria, R., Gallo, C., Bolso, M., dos Santos, B., Prisco, C. R., Bottino, C., & Ines, N. M. (2013). Comparative study of the uses of grammatical categories: Adjectives, adverbs, pronouns, interjections, conjunctions and prepositions in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: The Journal of the Alzheimer’s Association, 9(4), P882.

    Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Neurocognitive disorders. In Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: n.a. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.neurocognitivedisorders.

  • Atkinson, J. M., & Heritage, J. (Eds.). (1984). Structures of social action. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayles, K. A., Tomoeda, C. K., Kaszniak, A. W., Stern, L. Z., & Eagans, K. K. (1985). Verbal perseveration of dementia patients. Brain and Language, 25, 102–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berthier, M. L., Torres-Prioris, M. J., & LĂłpez-Barroso, D. (2017). Thinking on treating echolalia in aphasia: Recommendations and caveats for future research directions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00164.

  • Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., Finegan, E., & Quirk, R. (1999). Longman grammar of spoken and written English (Vol. 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 355–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K. (1990). Structure in language: Creating form in talk. American Psychologist, 45(11), 1221–1236. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.45.11.1221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., McLean, J. F., & Cleland, A. A. (2007). Syntactic alignment and participant role in dialogue. Cognition, 104(2), 163–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cruz, F. M. D. (2010). Verbal repetitions and palilalia in Alzheimer’s discourse. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 24(11), 848–858.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93(3), 283–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, L. J., & Joanette, Y. (1997). Analysis of conversational topic shifts: A multiple case study. Brain and Language, 58, 92–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of talk. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Hillis, A. E., Weintraub, S., Kertesz, A., Mendez, M., Cappa, S. E. E. A., … Manes, F. (2011). Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology, 76(11), 1006–1014.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heritage, J. (1984). A change-of-state token and aspects of its sequential placement. In J. M. Atkinson & J. Heritage (Eds.), Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis (pp. 299–345). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hier, D. B., Hagenlocker, D., & Schindler, A. G. (1985). Language disintegration in dementia: Effects of etiology and severity. Brain and Language, 25, 117–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jefferson, G. (2004). Glossary of transcript symbols with an introduction. Pragmatics and Beyond New Series, 125, 13–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kempler, D., Andersen, E. S., & Henderson, V. W. (1995). Linguistic and attentional contributions to anomia in Alzheimer’s disease: A comparison of off-line vs. on-line sentence processing. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, 8, 33–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kertesz, A., Jesso, S., Harciarek, M., Blair, M., & McMonagle, P. (2010). What is semantic dementia? A cohort study of diagnostic features and clinical boundaries. Archives of Neurology, 67, 483–489. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneurol.2010.55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larner, A. J. (2015, July–August). Neurological signs: Echo phenomena. ACNR, 15(3), 16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. I. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, M. C., Almor, A., Henderson, V. W., Kempler, D., & Andersen, E. S. (2001). Assessing working memory and language comprehension in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain and Language, 78(1), 17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meteyard, L., & Patterson, K. (2009). The relation between content and structure in language production: An analysis of speech errors in semantic dementia. Brain and Language, 110(3), 121–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MĂĽller, N., & Guendouzi, J. A. (2005). Order and disorder in conversation: Encounters with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 19(5), 393–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peelle, J. E., & Grossman, M. (2008). Language processing in frontotemporal dementia: A brief review. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(1), 18–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(4), 633–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piguet, O., Hornberger, M., Shelley, B. P., Kipps, C. M., & Hodges, J. R. (2009). Sensitivity of current criteria for the diagnosis of behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 72, 732–737.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pope, C., & Davis, B. H. (2011). Finding a balance: The Carolinas Conversation Collection. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 7(1), 143–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ripich, D. N., Vertes, D., Whitehouse, P., Fulton, S., & Ekelman, B. (1991). Turn-taking and speech act patterns in the discourse of senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type patients. Brain and Language, 40(3), 330–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ripich, D. N., & Terrell, B. Y. (1988). Patterns of discourse cohesion and coherence in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 53, 8–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42(1–3), 107–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, E. B., Bannister, K. A., & Anas, A. P. (2009). The dementia narrative: Writing to reclaim social identity. Journal of Aging Studies, 23(3), 145–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on conversation (2 Vols.; Fall 1964–Spring 1972). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schegloff, E. A. (2007). Sequence organization in interaction: A primer in conversation analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stickle, T., & Wanner, A. (2017). Transitivity patterns exhibited by persons with dementia in conversation. Applied Linguistics, 40(1), 43–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Recommended Readings

  • Guendouzi, J., & Muller, N. (2006). Approaches to discourse in dementia. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, H. E. (2005). Conversations with an Alzheimer’s patient: An interactional sociolinguistic study. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramanathan, V. (1997). Alzheimer discourse: Some sociolinguistic dimensions. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sabat, S. R. (2001). The experience of Alzheimer’s disease: Life through a tangled veil. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

Foundational Studies

  • Davis, B. H. (Ed.). (2005). Alzheimer talk, text and context: Enhancing communication. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

Emerging Research

  • Banovic, S., Zunic, L. J., & Sinanovic, O. (2018). Communication difficulties as a result of dementia. Materia Socio-Medica, 30(3), 221–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, B. H., & Maclagan, M. (2018). Narrative and ageing: Exploring the range of narrative types in dementia conversation. European Journal of English Studies, 22(1), 76–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leiros, B. G., MĂ©ndez, L. P., Huerta, M. Z., Eguinoa, L. M., GarcĂ­a-Bragado, F., Alvarez, T. T., & Larreta, J. R. (2018). Prevalence and concordance between the clinical and the post-mortem diagnosis of dementia in a psychogeriatric clinic. NeurologĂ­a (English Edition), 33(1), 13–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGilton, K. S., Rochon, E., Sidani, S., Shaw, A., Ben-David, B. M., Saragosa, M., … Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2017). Can we help care providers communicate more effectively with persons having dementia living in long-term care homes? American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease & Other Dementias, 32(1), 41–50.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Trini Stickle .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Stickle, T., Wanner, A. (2020). Making Sense of Syntactic Error in Conversations Between Persons with Dementia and Their Non-impaired Co-participants. In: Stickle, T. (eds) Learning from the Talk of Persons with Dementia. The Language of Mental Health. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-43977-4_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics