The Social Nature of Emotion Expression pp 141-160 | Cite as
Inferring Intentions from Emotion Expressions in Social Decision Making
- 426 Downloads
Abstract
In the last decade we have seen increasing experimental evidence that people make important inferences from emotion expressions about others’ intentions in situations of interdependent decision making. Reverse appraisal has been proposed as one mechanism whereby people retrieve, from emotion displays, information about how others are appraising the ongoing interaction (e.g., does my counterpart find the current outcome to be goal conducive? Does s/he blame me for it?); in turn, from these appraisal attributions, people make inferences about the others’ goals (e.g., is my counterpart likely to cooperate?) that shape their decision making. Here we review experimental evidence and progress that has been done in understanding this inferential mechanism and its relationship to other mechanisms for the interpersonal effects of emotion (e.g., emotional contagion and social appraisal). We discuss theoretical implications for our understanding of the role of emotion expression on human decision making, but also practical implications for the growing industry of socially intelligent machines (e.g., personal digital assistants and social robots).
Keywords
Emotional expression Reverse appraisal Theory-of-mind Automatic expression recognitionReferences
- Alfonso, B., Pynadath, D., Lhommet, M., & Marsella, S. (2013). Emotional perception for updating agents’ beliefs. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII). https://doi.org/10.1016/2Fj.tics.2012.07.009.
- Al-Fuqaha, A., Guizani, M., Mohammadi, M., Aledhari, M., & Ayyash, M. (2015). Internet of things: A survey on enabling technologies, protocols, and applications. IEEE Communication Surveys & Tutorials, 17, 2347–2376. https://doi.org/10.1109/COMST.2015.2444095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Algoe, S. B., Buswell, B. N., & DeLamater, J. D. (2000). Gender and job status as contextual cues for the interpretation of facial expression of emotion. Sex Roles, 42, 183–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Amodio, D., & Frith, C. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 268–277. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1884.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Andrade, E., & Ho, T.-H. (2009). Gaming emotions in social interactions. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 539–551. https://doi.org/10.1086/599221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Aviezer, H., Hassin, R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., et al. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19, 724–732. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02148.x.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Baltrušaitis, T., Ahuja, C., & Morency, L. P. (2018). Multimodal machine learning: A survey and taxonomy. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence. https://doi.org/10.1109/TPAMI.2018.2798607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42, 241–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Boone, R., & Buck, R. (2003). Emotional expressivity and trustworthiness: The role of nonverbal behavior in the evolution of cooperation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27, 163–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Breazeal, C. (2003). Toward sociable robots. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 42, 167–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0921-8890(02)00373-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brown, W., Palameta, B., & Moore, C. (2003). Are there nonverbal cues to commitment? An exploratory study using the zero-acquaintance video presentation paradigm. Evolutionary Psychology, 1, 42–69. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F147470490300100104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Calvo, R., & D’Mello, S. (2010). Affect detection: An interdisciplinary review of models, methods, and their applications. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 1, 16–37. https://doi.org/10.1109/T-AFFC.2010.1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Camerer, C. (2003). Psychology and economics. Strategizing in the brain. Science, 300, 1673–1675. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1086215.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Côté, S., Hideg, I., & van Kleef, G. (2013). The consequences of faking anger in negotiations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 453–463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.12.015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- D’Mello, S., & Kory, J. (2015). A review and meta-analysis of multimodal affect detection systems. ACM Computing Surveys, 47. https://doi.org/10.1145/2682899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dally, M., Medasani, S., Behringer, R., & Trivedl, M. (2017). Self-driving cars. IEEE Computer, 50, 18–23. https://doi.org/10.1109/MC.2017.4451204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. New York: Putnam.Google Scholar
- Dawes, R. (1980). Social dilemmas. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 169–193. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.31.020180.001125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- de Melo, C., Carnevale, P., Read, S., Antos, D., & Gratch, J. (2012). Bayesian model of the social effects of emotion in decision-making in multiagent systems. In: Proceedings of Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (AAMAS 12).Google Scholar
- de Melo, C., Carnevale, P., & Gratch, J. (2014a). Using virtual confederates to research intergroup bias and conflict. Academy of Management Proceedings, 2014(1).Google Scholar
- de Melo, C., Carnevale, P., Read, S., & Gratch, J. (2014b). Reading people’s minds from emotion expressions in interdependent decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 73–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- de Melo, C., Marsella, S., & Gratch, J. (2019). Human cooperation when acting through autonomous machines. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 116, 3482–3487. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1817656116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- DeSteno, D., Breazeal, C., Frank, R., Pizarro, D., Baumann, J., Dickens, L., et al. (2012). Detecting the trustworthiness of novel partners in economic exchange. Psychological Science, 23, 1549–1556. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F0956797612448793.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ekman, P. (1993). Facial expression and emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 384–392. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0003-066X.48.4.384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0030377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ellsworth, P., & Scherer, K. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In R. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572–595). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Farrell, J., & Rabin, M. (1996). Cheap talk. Journal of Economics Perspectives, 10, 103–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fischer, A., Rotteveel, M., Evers, C., & Manstead, A. (2004). Emotional assimilation: How we are influenced by others’ emotions. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 22, 223–245.Google Scholar
- Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: An emotion script approach to anger episodes between workers and their superiors, co-workers and subordinates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 147–162. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(200003)21:2%3C147::AID-JOB35%3E3.0.CO;2-T.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Floreano, D., & Wood, R. (2015). Science, technology and the future of small autonomous drones. Nature, 521, 460–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Frank, R. (1988). Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
- Frank, R. (2004). Introducing moral emotions into models of rational choice. In A. Manstead, N. Frijda, & A. Fischer (Eds.), Feelings and emotions (pp. 422–440). New York: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511806582.024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Frijda, N. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Frijda, N., & Mesquita, B. (1994). The social roles and functions of emotions. In S. Kitayama & H. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence (pp. 51–87). Washington: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10152-002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Frijda, N., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–228. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.2.212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gallese, V. (2007). Before and below ‘theory of mind’: Embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philosophical Transactions Royal Social London B Biological Science, 362, 659–669. https://doi.org/10.1098/2Frstb.2006.2002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gratch, J., Rickel, J., Andre, E., Badler, N., Cassell, J., & Petajan, E. (2002). Creating interactive virtual humans: Some assembly required. IEEE Intelligent Systems, 17, 54–63. https://doi.org/10.1109/MIS.2002.1024753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gunnery, S., Hall, J., & Ruben, M. (2013). The deliberate Duchenne smile: Individual differences in expressive control. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37, 29–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Haidt, J. (2003). Autonomic specificity and emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852–870). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Hareli, S. (2014). Making sense of the social world and influencing it by using a naïve attribution theory of emotions. Emotion Review, 6, 336–343. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914534501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hareli, S., & Hess, U. (2010). What emotional reactions can tell us about the nature of others: An appraisal perspective on person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 128–140. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802613828.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hareli, S., Elkabetz, S., & Hess, U. (2019). Drawing inferences from emotion expressions: The role of situative informativeness and context. Emotion. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapson, R. (1994). Emotional contagion. New York: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1080/00029157.1997.10403399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2016). The impact of context on the perception of emotions. In C. Abell & J. Smith (Eds.), The expression of emotion: Philosophical, psychological, and legal perspectives (pp. 199–218). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1986.62.1.79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hess, U., & Hareli, S. (2018). On the malleability of the meaning of contexts: The influence of another person’s emotion expressions on situation perception. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 185–191. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2016.1269725.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Hess, U., Blaison, C., & Kafetsios, K. (2016). Judging facial emotion expressions in context: The influence of culture and self-construal orientation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40, 50–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ickes, W., & Simpson, J. A. (2004). Motivational aspects of empathic accuracy. In M. B. Brewer & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Emotion and motivation. Perspectives on social psychology (pp. 225–246). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
- Kachouie, R., Sedighadeli, S., Khosla, R., & Chu, M.-T. (2014). Socially assistive robots in elderly care: A mixed-method systematic literature review. International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction, 30, 369–393. https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2013.873278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Keltner, D., & Ekman, P. (2000). Facial expression of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 236–249). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
- Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (1999). Social functions of emotions at four levels of analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 505–521. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999399379168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Keltner, D., & Kring, A. M. (1998). Emotion, social function, and psychopathology. Review of General Psychology, 2, 320–342. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kott, A., Swami, A., & West, B. (2016). The internet of battle things. IEEE Computer, 49, 70–75. https://doi.org/10.1109/MC.2016.355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kreibig, S. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84, 394–421. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Krumhuber, E. G., & Manstead, A. S. (2009). Can Duchenne smiles be feigned? New evidence on felt and false smiles. Emotion, 9(6), 807. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A., & Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion, 7, 730–735. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Krumhuber, E., Kappas, A., & Manstead, A. (2013). Effects of dynamic aspects of facial expressions: A review. Emotion Review, 5, 41–46. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F1754073912451349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lanzetta, J., & Englis, B. (1989). Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observer’s vicarious emotional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 543–554. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.56.4.543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lazarus, R. (1991). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 819–834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lerner, J., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., & Kassam, K. (2015). Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 799–823. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Levenson, R. (2003). Autonomic specificity and emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 212–224). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. (1987). The relationship of emotion to cognition: A functional approach to a semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 3–28. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938708408361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lewis, M. (2001). Personal pathways in the development of appraisal: A complex systems/stage theory perspective. In K. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 205–220). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Lin, R., & Kraus, S. (2010). Can automated agents proficiently negotiate with humans? Communications of the ACM, 53, 78–88. https://doi.org/10.1145/1629175.1629199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In R. Davidson, K. Scherer, & H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 619–642). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- López, G., Quesada, L., & Guerrero, L. (2017). Alexa vs. Siri vs. Cortana vs. Google Assistant: A comparison of speech-based natural user interfaces. In Proceedings of International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics (AHFE 2017).Google Scholar
- Manstead, A., & Fischer, A. (2001). Social appraisal: The social world as object of and influence on appraisal processes. In K. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 221–232). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Mehu, M., Grammer, K., & Dunbar, R. (2007). Smiles when sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 415–422. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.05.010.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Mesquita, B., & Ellsworth, P. (2001). The role of culture in appraisal. In K. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 233–248). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Morris, M., & Keltner, D. (2000). How emotions work: An analysis of the social functions of emotional expression in negotiations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 22, 1–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-3085(00)22002-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Niedenthal, P., Mermillod, M., Maringer, M., & Hess, U. (2010). The simulation of smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 417–480. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ortony, A., Clore, G., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Parkinson, B. (2019). Calibrating emotional orientations: Social appraisal and other kinds of relation alignment. In D. Dukes & F. Clément (Eds.), Foundations of affective social learning: Conceptualising the transmission of social value. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Parkinson, B., & Simons, G. (2009). Affecting others: Social appraisal and emotion contagion in everyday decision making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1071–1084. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F0146167209336611.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Petta, P., Marsella, S., & Gratch, J. (2010). Computational models of emotion. In K. Scherer, T. Bänziger, & E. Roesch (Eds.), A blueprint for affective computing: A sourcebook and manual (pp. 21–40). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Picard, R. (1997). Affective computing. Cambridge: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Poletti, M., Enrici, I., & Adenzato, M. (2012). Cognitive and affective theory of mind in neurodegenerative diseases: Neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 2147–2164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Preacher, K., & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Reisenzein, R. (2009). Emotions as metarepresentational states of mind: Naturalizing the belief–desire theory of emotion. Cognitive Systems Research, 10, 6–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.03.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Rens, H., Stratou, G., & Gratch, J. (2017). Incorporating emotion perception into opponent modeling for social dilemmas. In Proceedings of the International Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (AAMAS).Google Scholar
- Rilling, J., & Sanfey, A. (2011). The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 23–48. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131647.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Roseman, I. (2001). A model of appraisal in the emotion system: Integrating theory, research, and applications. In K. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 68–91). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Sariyanidi, E., Gunes, H., & Cavallaro, A. (2015). Automatic analysis of facial affect: A survey of registration, representation, and recognition. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 37, 1113–1133. https://doi.org/10.1109/TPAMI.2014.2366127.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Scharlemann, J., Eckel, C., Kacelnik, A., & Wilson, R. (2001). The value of a smile: Game theory with a human face. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 617–640. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4870(01)00059-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Scherer, K. (2001). Appraisal considered as a process of multi-level sequential checking. In K. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 92–120). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Scherer, K. (2010). The component process model: Architecture for a comprehensive computational model of emergent emotion. In K. Scherer, T. Bänziger, & E. Roesch (Eds.), A blueprint for affective computing: A sourcebook and manual (pp. 47–70). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Scherer, K., & Grandjean, D. (2008). Facial expressions allow inference of both emotions and their components. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 789–801. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701516791.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Scherer, K., & Moors, A. (2019). The emotion process: Event appraisal and component differentiation. Annual Review of Psychology, 70. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Scherer, K., Johnstone, T., & Klasmeyer, G. (2003). Vocal expression of emotion. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 433–456). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Scherer, K., Mortillaro, M., Rotondi, I., Sergi, I., & Trznadel, S. (2018). Appraisal-driven facial actions as building blocks for emotion inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114, 358–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000107.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Schug, J., Matsumoto, D., Horita, Y., Yamagishi, T., & Bonnet, K. (2010). Emotional expressivity as a signal of cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 87–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.09.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shamay-Tsoory, S. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. The Neuroscientist, 17, 18–24. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858410379268.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Shamay-Tsoory, S., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2007). Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: A lesion study. Neuropsychologia, 45, 3054–3067. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.021.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Shamay-Tsoory, S., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2009). Two systems for empathy: A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 132, 617–627. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awn279.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Shore, D., & Parkinson, B. (2018). Interpersonal effects of strategic and spontaneous guilt communication in trust games. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 1382–1390. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1395728.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sorce, J. F., Emde, R. N., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. D. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1 year olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195–200. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.1.195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Spencer, S., Zanna, M., & Fong, G. (2005). Establishing a causal chain: Why experiments are often more effective than mediational analyses in examining psychological processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 845–851. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.6.845.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Stone, R., & Lavine, M. (2014). The social life of robots. Science, 346, 178–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Stratou, G., Hoegen, R., Lucas, G., & Gratch, J. (2015). Emotional signaling in a social dilemma: An automatic analysis. In: Proceedings of the International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (ACII).Google Scholar
- Sutton, R. (1991). Maintaining norms about expressed emotions: The case of bill collectors. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 245–268. https://doi.org/10.2307/2393355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Tamir, M., Mitchell, C., & Gross, J. (2008). Hedonic and instrumental motives in anger regulation. Psychological Science, 19, 324–328. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02088.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Terada, K., & Takeuchi, C. (2017). Emotional expression in simple line drawings of a robot’s face leads to higher offers in the ultimatum game. Frontiers of Psychology, 8, 724. https://doi.org/10.3389/2Ffpsyg.2017.00724.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- van Dijk, E., van Kleef, G. A., Steinel, W., & van Beest, I. (2008). A social functional approach to emotions in bargaining: When communicating anger pays and when it backfires. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 600–614. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.600.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- van Kleef, G. (2016). The interpersonal dynamics of emotion: Toward an integrative theory of emotions as social information. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- van Kleef, G., De Dreu, C., & Manstead, A. (2004). The interpersonal effects of anger and happiness in negotiations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 57–76. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.305070.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- van Kleef, G., De Dreu, C., & Manstead, A. (2006). Supplication and appeasement in negotiation: The interpersonal effects of disappointment, worry, guilt, and regret. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 124–142. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.124.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- van Kleef, G., De Dreu, C., & Manstead, A. (2010). An interpersonal approach to emotion in social decision making: The emotions as social information model. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 45–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(10)42002-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- van Kleef, G., Homan, A., & Cheshin, A. (2012). Emotional influence at work: Take it EASI. Organizational Psychology Review, 2, 311–339. https://doi.org/10.1177/2F2041386612454911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Waldrop, M. (2015). No drivers required. Nature, 518, 20–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Weiner, B. (1987). The social psychology of emotion: Applications of a naive psychology. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5, 405–419. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1987.5.4.405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wu, Y., Baker, C., Tenenbaum, J., & Schulz, L. (2018). Rational inference of beliefs and desires from emotional expressions. Cognitive Science, 42, 850–884. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.1987.5.4.405.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Zeng, Z., Pantic, M., Roisman, G., & Huang, T. (2009). A survey of affect recognition methods: Audio, visual, and spontaneous expressions. IEEE Transactions on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence, 31, 39–58. https://doi.org/10.1109/TPAMI.2008.52.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar