Skip to main content

Neuroscience and the New Psychologies: Epistemological First Aid

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Psychological Studies of Science and Technology

Abstract

Critics of neuroscientific developments in psychology have emphasized the reductionist impulse underlying these advances and their adherence to a mereological fallacy wherein powers are attributed to brains when these can only be ascribed to persons as a whole. These critiques among others, while important, miss the crucial features of a neuroscience that will alter our understanding of both human complexity and the emergence of our most characteristic attributes. Following Rose and Abi-Rached, the picture of our brain as plastic and ultimately social is a revisionist one. This chapter argues that rather than creating a sharp division between a reductionist technoscience on the one hand, and a militant subjectivity on the other, the brain question makes possible the articulation of multiple frames. A technoscience stance asks that what there is to know of the brain is neither to be feared as reductionist nor limiting for subjectivities but instead melds into new forms of theorizing. I do this by converting Solymosi’s (and Dennett’s) conception of “ethical first aid” to the construct of an “epistemological first aid” that sets out an ersatz manual for understanding the place of the neurosciences in our activities of theorizing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Solymosi is here referring to Larry Hickman, who argues that technoscience is the more appropriate term for what is nominally called “science” since it is in fact a branch of technology that involves the use of tools and artifacts that are employed on raw materials to solve problems (see Hickman, 2001). Science does no more come before technology than it is separate from it. Techoscience is on this account continuous with human experience.

  2. 2.

    This is actually controversial, like so many findings in social psychology. The results of multiple studies have complicated the original findings and there is no overall theoretical model to give an account of the findings that are reported (Fischer et al., 2011). However, the results of these studies have become embedded in the discourse of “bystanders” in North America. For example, my university offers training to faculty and students in “bystander intervention.” I say this not to critique such training, which undoubtedly is useful for some ends, but to note how the notion of the inactive bystander has become a common trope.

  3. 3.

    It could be argued of course that there was no such thing as the “bystander effect” in the nineteenth century because the historical, urban conditions of alienation that led to this effect were not yet fully present. Hence no phrenological account could be provided.

References

  • Anderson, M. L. (2014). After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, D. (1974). The coming of post-industrial society. New York: Harper Colophon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, M. R., Dennett, D., Hacker, P. M. S., Searle, J., & Robinson, D. (2007). Neuroscience and philosophy: Brain, mind, and language. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, M. R., & Hacker, P. M. S. (2003). Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catani, M. (2017). A little man of some importance. Brain,140, 3055–3061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury, S., & Slaby, J. (2012). Critical neuroscience: A handbook of the social and cultural contexts of neuroscience. Chichester: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coey, C. A., Varlet, M., & Richardson, M. J. (2012). Coordination dynamics in a socially situated nervous system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,6, 164. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,8, 377–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Rijcke, S., & Beaulieu, A. (2007). Essay review: Taking a good look at why scientific images don’t speak for themselves. Theory & Psychology,17, 733–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (1988). The moral first aid manual. In M. S. McMurrin (Ed.), Tanner lectures on human values (Vol. VIII, pp. 120–147). Salt Lake City: University of Utah Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dumit, J. (2004). Picturing personhood: Brain scans and biomedical identity. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dupré, J. (2001). Human nature and the limits of science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, P., Krueger, J. I., Greitemeyer, T., Vogrincic, C., Kastenmüller, A., Frey, D., et al. (2011). The bystander-effect: A meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies. Psychological Bulletin,137, 517–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, J. A. (2013, July 1). Gut feelings: Bacteria and the brain. Cerebrum. Retrieved from http://www.dana.org/Cerebrum/Default.aspx?id=39496.

  • Furr, N., Nel, K., & Ramsøy, T. Z. (2019). Neuroscience is going to change how businesses understand their customers. Harvard Business Review. Retrieved from https://hbr.org/2019/02/neuroscience-is-going-to-change-how-businesses-understand-their-customers.

  • Gergen, K. J. (2010). The acculturated brain. Theory & Psychology,20, 795–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (1991). From tools to theories: A heuristic of discovery in cognitive psychology. Psychological Review,98, 254–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J., & Cohen, J. (2004). For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences,359, 1775–1785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2012, October 1). Jonathan Haidt on moral psychology. Social Science Space. Retrieved from https://www.socialsciencespace.com/2012/10/jonathan-haidt-on-moral-psychology/.

  • Hickman, L. A. (2001). Philosophical tools for technological culture: Putting pragmatism to work. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hortensius, R., & de Gelder, B. (2014). The neural basis of the bystander effect—The influence of group size on neural activity when witnessing an emergency. Neuroimage, 93(Pt1), 53–58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1934). The concept of the habit-family hierarchy and maze learning: Part I. Psychological Review,41, 33–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jordan-Young, R., & Rumiati, R. I. (2012). Hardwired for sexism? Approaches to sex/gender in neuroscience. Neuroethics,5, 305–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1990). Technology is society made durable. The Sociological Review,38(S1), 103–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (2004). How to talk about the body? The normative dimension of science studies. Body & Society,10, 205–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madroñal, N., et al. (2016). Rapid erasure of hippocampal memory following inhibition of dentate gyrus granule cells. Nature Communications, 7. Article no. 10923. Retrieved from https://www.nature.com/articles/ncomms10923.

  • Margolis, J. (1984). Philosophy of psychology. New York: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrath, M. (2019, March 28). Good Samaritans after all. Aeon. Retrieved from https://aeon.co/essays/it-looks-like-human-beings-might-be-good-samaritans-after-all?utm_medium=feed&utm_source=rss-feed.

  • Panchin, A. Y., Tuzhikov, A., & Panchin, Y. V. (2014). Midichlorians—The biomeme hypothesis: Is there a microbial component to religious rituals? Biology Direct, 9. Retrieved from http://www.biologydirect.com/content/9/1/14.

  • Penfield, W., & Boldrev, E. (1937). Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain,60, 389–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Penfield, W., & Rasmussen, T. (1950). The cerebral cortex of man. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radder, H., & Meyen, G. (2012). Does the brain “initiate” freely willed processes? A philosophy of science critique of Libet-type experiments and their interpretation. Theory & Psychology,23, 3–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. (1973). The enlightened machine: An analytical introduction to neuropsychology. New York: Dickinson.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Roiser, J. (2015). What has neuroscience ever done for us? The Psychologist,28, 284–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rose, N., & Abi-Rached, J. M. (2013). Neuro: The new brain sciences and the management of the mind. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rose, N., O’Malley, P., & Valverde, M. (2009, September 16). Governmentality. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 2, 83–104. Sydney Law School Research Paper No. 09/94. Available at SSRN https://ssrn.com/abstract=1474131.

  • Schieber, M. H. (2001). Constraints on somatotopic organization in the primary motor cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology,86, 2125–2143.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, S., & Höppner, G. (2014). Neurofeminism and feminist neurosciences: A critical review of contemporary brain research. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. Article no. 546. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00546.

  • Schott, G. D. (1993). Penfield’s homunculus: A note on cerebral cartography. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry,56, 329–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. J., Lilienfeld, S. O., Meca, A., & Sauvigné, K. C. (2016). The role of neuroscience within psychology: A call for inclusiveness over exclusiveness. American Psychologist,71, 52–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R. (2010). The will: Victorian and modern responses to the brain sciences. Paper presented at the Symposium on Neuroscience and Human Nature, Wellcome Trust Centre for the History of Medicine at UCL. Retrieved from http://www.rogersmith.ru/papers.

  • Solymosi, T. (2014). Moral first aid for a neuroscientific age. In T. Solymosi & J. R. Shook (Eds.), Neuroscience, neurophilosophy and pragmatism: Brains at work with the world (pp. 291–317). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stam, H. J. (2015). The neurosciences and the search for a unified psychology: The science and esthetics of a single framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. Article no. 1467. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01467.

  • Useem, J. (2017, July/August). Power causes brain damage: How leaders lose mental capacities—Most notably for reading other people—That were essential to their rise. The Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/2017/07/power-causes-brain-damage/528711/.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Henderikus J. Stam .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Stam, H.J. (2019). Neuroscience and the New Psychologies: Epistemological First Aid. In: O’Doherty, K.C., Osbeck, L.M., Schraube, E., Yen, J. (eds) Psychological Studies of Science and Technology. Palgrave Studies in the Theory and History of Psychology. Palgrave Macmillan, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-25308-0_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics