Advertisement

Free Choice

  • Davood Gozli
Chapter
  • 249 Downloads

Abstract

This chapter addresses experimental research on “free-choice” responses. A free-choice response, in this approach, is operationally defined as the arbitrary selection of one response from multiple alternatives, contrasted with a “forced-choice” response, which is defined as the selection of one correct response given unambiguous rules and stimuli. I argue, first, that by defining actions and decisions, in advance, as free, we are left with a limited range of research questions (“Can free-choice actions be influenced?” “Why do free-choice actions take more time?”), answers to which are to a large extent pre-determined by our operational definitions and experimental procedures. Second, the operational definition of free-choice response, in these experiments, is grounded in a particular philosophical view of freedom that is based on detachment, withdrawal, and negation; which is by no means the only way (or the best way) to think about human freedom. Third, in practice, experimental research does not even meet the requirements of its own conception of free choice. In practice, free-choice responses are quasi-forced-choice actions, subject to standards of evaluation and inclusion. Empirical findings ought to be considered in light of these limitations.

Keywords

Experimental psychology Free choice Forced choice Autonomy Intention Stimulus-based response Intention-based response Volition Randomness Normativity Participation Construct validity 

References

  1. Ansorge, U. (2002). Spatial intention-response compatibility. Acta Psychologica, 109(3), 285–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Berlin, I. (2002). Two concepts of liberty. In H. Hardy (Ed.), Liberty. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berlyne, D. E. (1957). Conflict and choice time. British Journal of Psychology, 48, 106–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bermeitinger, C., & Hackländer, R. P. (2018). Response priming with motion primes: Negative compatibility or congruency effects, even in free-choice trials. Cognitive Processing, 19, 351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dennett, D. C. (2004). Freedom evolves. New York, NY: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  8. Dreisbach, G. (2012). Mechanisms of cognitive control: The functional role of task rules. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 227–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dreisbach, G., & Fröber, K. (2018). On how to be flexible (or not): Modulation of the stability-flexibility balance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28, 3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2001). Effect anticipation and action control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 229–240.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Elsner, B., & Hommel, B. (2004). Contiguity and contingency in the acquisition of action effects. Psychological Research, 68, 138–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fagioli, S., Hommel, B., & Schubotz, R. I. (2007). Intentional control of attention: Action planning primes action-related stimulus dimensions. Psychological Research, 71(1), 22–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Fischer, M. H. (2001). Number processing induces spatial performance biases. Neurology, 57(5), 822–826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Frith, C. (2013). The psychology of volition. Experimental Brain Research, 229, 289–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gozli, D. G. (2017). Behaviour versus performance: The veiled commitment of experimental psychology. Theory & Psychology, 27, 741–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gozli, D. G., & Ansorge, U. (2016). Action selection as a guide for visual attention. Visual Cognition, 24, 38–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Gozli, D. G., Huffman, G., & Pratt, J. (2016). Acting and anticipating: Impact of outcome-compatible distractor depends on response selection efficiency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1601–1614.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Haggard, P. (2008). Human volition: Towards a neuroscience of will. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 934–946.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Heinemann, A., Pfister, R., & Janczyk, M. (2013). Manipulating number generation: Loud + long = large? Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1332–1339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Herwig, A., Prinz, W., & Waszak, F. (2007). Two modes of sensorimotor integration in intention-based and stimulus-based actions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60(11), 1540–1554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Herwig, A., & Waszak, F. (2009). Intention and attention in ideomotor learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(2), 219–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hommel, B. (1996). The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects. Psychological Research, 59, 176–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hommel, B. (1997). Toward an action-concept model of stimulus-response compatibility. In B. Hommel & W. Prinz (Eds.), Theoretical issues in stimulus-response compatibility (pp. 281–320). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hommel, B. (1998). Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1368–1384.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and control in stimulus-response translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Control of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII (pp. 247–273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  26. Hommel, B. (2013). Ideomotor action control: On the perceptual grounding of voluntary actions and agents. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline (pp. 113–136). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  27. Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Huffman, G., Gozli, D. G., Hommel, B., & Pratt, J. (2018). Response preparation, response selection difficulty, and response-outcome learning. Psychological Research, 83, 247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hughes, G., Schütz-Bosbach, S., & Waszak, F. (2011). One action system or two? Evidence for common central preparatory mechanisms in voluntary and stimulus-driven actions. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(46), 16692–16699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Janczyk, M., Dambacher, M., Bieleke, M., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2015). The benefit of no choice: Goal-directed plans enhance perceptual processing. Psychological Research, 79, 206–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Janczyk, M., & Kunde, W. (2014). The role of effect grouping in free-choice response selection. Acta Psychologica, 150, 49–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Janczyk, M., Nolden, S., & Jolicoeur, P. (2015). No differences in dual-task costs between forced-and free-choice tasks. Psychological Research, 79, 463–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Khan, M. A., Mourton, S., Buckolz, E., Adam, J. J., & Hayes, A. E. (2010). The influence of response grouping on free-choice decision making in a response selection task. Acta Psychologica, 134, 175–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Kiesel, A., Wagener, A., Kunde, W., Hoffmann, J., Fallgatter, A. J., & Stöcker, C. (2006). Unconscious manipulation of free choice in humans. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 397–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Koch, I., & Kunde, W. (2002). Verbal response-effect compatibility. Memory & Cognition, 30(8), 1297–1303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Kukla, A., & Walmsley, J. (2006). Mind: A historical and philosophical introduction to the major theories. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing.Google Scholar
  37. Kunde, W. (2001). Response-effect compatibility in manual choice reaction tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 387–394.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Künzell, S., Broeker, L., Dignath, D., Ewolds, H., Raab, M., & Thomaschke, R. (2018). What is a task? An ideomotor perspective. Psychological Research, 82, 4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Marcel, G. (1962). Man against mass society (G. S. Frasier, Trans.). South Bend, IN: St. Augustine Press.Google Scholar
  40. Marken, R. S. (2014). Doing research on purpose: A control theory approach to experimental psychology. Chapel Hill, NC: New View.Google Scholar
  41. Mattler, U., & Palmer, S. (2012). Time course of free-choice priming effects explained by a simple accumulator model. Cognition, 123(3), 347–360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Naefgen, C., Caissie, A. F., & Janczyk, M. (2017). Stimulus-response links and the backward crosstalk effect—A comparison of forced-and free-choice tasks. Acta Psychologica, 177, 23–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Naefgen, C., Dambacher, M., & Janczyk, M. (2018). Why free choices take longer than forced choices: Evidence from response threshold manipulations. Psychological Research, 82, 1039.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Naefgen, C., & Janczyk, M. (2018). Free choice tasks as random generation tasks: An investigation through working memory manipulations. Experimental Brain Research., 236, 2263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Pfister, R., Janczyk, M., Gressmann, M., Fournier, L. R., & Kunde, W. (2014). Good vibrations? Vibrotactile self-stimulation reveals anticipation of body-related action effects in motor control. Experimental Brain Research, 232(3), 847–854.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Hoffmann, J. (2011). Learning at any rate: Action-effect learning for stimulus-based actions. Psychological Research, 75(1), 61–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Pfister, R., Kiesel, A., & Melcher, T. (2010). Adaptive control of ideomotor effect anticipations. Acta Psychologica, 135(3), 316–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Ristic, J., & Kingstone, A. (2006). Attention to arrows: Pointing to a new direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(11), 1921–1930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of arm, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(4), 444–474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Sartre, J. P. (1956). Being and nothingness. New York, NY: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
  51. Schlaghecken, F., & Eimer, M. (2004). Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 463–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Shin, Y. K., Proctor, R. W., & Capaldi, E. J. (2010). A review of contemporary ideomotor theory. Psychological Bulletin, 136(6), 943–974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Smedslund, J. (1991). The pseudoempirical in psychology and the case for psychologic. Psychological Inquiry, 2, 325–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Smedslund, J. (1997). The structure of psychological common sense. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  55. Smedslund, J. (2002). From hypothesis-testing psychology to procedure-testing psychologic. Review of General Psychology, 6(1), 51–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Stam, H. (1990). What distinguishes lay persons’ psychological explanations from those of psychologists? In W. J. Baker, M. E. Hyland, R. van Hezewijk, & S. Terwee (Eds.), Recent trends in theoretical psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 97–106). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Strawson, P. F. (1992). Analysis and metaphysics. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Verschuere, B., Prati, V., & Houwer, J. D. (2009). Cheating the lie detector: Faking in the autobiographical Implicit Association Test. Psychological Science, 20(4), 410–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Wolfensteller, U., & Ruge, H. (2011). On the timescale of stimulus-based action–effect learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(7), 1273–1289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Wolfensteller, U., & Ruge, H. (2014). Response selection difficulty modulates the behavioral impact of rapidly learnt action effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Wykowska, A., Schubö, A., & Hommel, B. (2009). How you move is what you see: Action planning biases selection in visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(6), 1755–1769.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Ziessler, M., & Nattkemper, D. (2011). The temporal dynamics of effect anticipation in course of action planning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(7), 1305–1326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Ziessler, M., Nattkemper, D., & Frensch, P. A. (2004). The role of anticipation and intention in the learning of effects of self-performed actions. Psychological Research, 68(2-3), 163–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Ziessler, M., Nattkemper, D., & Vogt, S. (2012). The activation of effect codes in response preparation: New evidence from an indirect priming paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Zwosta, K., Ruge, H., & Wolfensteller, U. (2013). No anticipation without intention: Response–effect compatibility in effect-based and stimulus-based actions. Acta Psychologica, 144(3), 628–634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Davood Gozli
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of MacauTaipaMacao

Personalised recommendations