Advertisement

Developing Disgust: Theory, Measurement, and Application

  • Joshua RottmanEmail author
  • Jasmine DeJesus
  • Heather Greenebaum
Chapter

Abstract

Disgust is a complex and uncharacteristic emotion. Despite being frequently classified as a “basic” emotion, disgust has a wide range of elicitors, many competing functional theories, and a protracted developmental trajectory. This chapter first reviews several ultimate explanations of disgust, highlighting how scholars historically privileged symbolic explanations, while most contemporary researchers believe disgust to be an adaptive pathogen avoidance mechanism. After a brief discussion of techniques for measuring disgust, we describe the current knowledge of the development of disgust, with special attention to the ways in which disgust influences food choice and contributes to contamination sensitivity. While certain aspects of disgust may be universal, its emergence is largely enculturated and its expression is highly variable. We conclude by discussing the ways in which the study of disgust carries practical implications for the diagnosis and treatment of psychopathologies, for nutrition, and for the implementation of public health initiatives. Although scholarly interest in disgust has greatly increased during recent years, there is still much room for further exploration of this enigmatic emotion.

Keywords

Disgust Development Childhood Avoidance Disease Emotion Food 

References

  1. Aboud, F. E. (2003). The formation of in-group favoritism and out-group prejudice in young children: Are they distinct attitudes? Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 48–60.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  2. Ammann, J., Hartmann, C., & Siegrist, M. (2018). Development and validation of the Food Disgust Picture Scale. Appetite, 125, 367–379.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  3. Angyal, A. (1941). Disgust and related aversions. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 36, 393–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Askew, C., Çakır, K., Põldsam, L., & Reynolds, G. (2014). The effect of disgust and fear modeling on children’s disgust and fear for animals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 123(3), 566–577.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Au, T. K., Sidle, A. L., & Rollins, K. B. (1993). Developing an intuitive understanding of conservation and contamination: Invisible particles as a plausible mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 286–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Aviezer, H., Hassin, R. R., Ryan, J., Grady, C., Susskind, J., Anderson, A., … Bentin, S. (2008). Angry, disgusted, or afraid? Studies on the malleability of emotion perception. Psychological Science, 19(7), 724–732.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  7. Barrett, L. F. (2017). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
  8. Becker, E. (1973). The denial of death. New York, NY: Free Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bernstein, I. L. (1978). Learned taste aversions in children receiving chemotherapy. Science, 200, 1302–1303.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  10. Bernstein, I. L. (1994). Development of food aversions during illness. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, 53, 131–137.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Birch, L. L. (1990). Development of food acceptance patterns. Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 515–519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Birch, L. L. (1999). Development of food preferences. Annual Review of Nutrition, 19(1), 41–62.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Blacker, K.-A., & LoBue, V. (2016). Behavioral avoidance of contagion in childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 143, 162–170.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  14. Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  15. Brady, R. E., Adams, T. G., & Lohr, J. M. (2010). Disgust in contamination-based obsessive-compulsive disorder: A review and model. Expert Review of Neurotherapeutics, 10(8), 1295–1305.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Brown, S. D., & Harris, G. (2012). Disliked food acting as a contaminant during infancy. A disgust based motivation for rejection. Appetite, 58(2), 535–538.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Brown, S. D., Harris, G., Bell, L., & Lines, L. M. (2012). Disliked food acting as a contaminant in a sample of young children. Appetite, 58(3), 991–996.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  18. Bryce, J., Boschi-Pinto, C., Shibuya, K., Black, R. E., & the WHO Child Health Epidemiology Reference Group. (2005). WHO estimates of the causes of death in children. Lancet, 365, 1147–1152.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  19. Buttelmann, D., & Böhm, R. (2014). The ontogeny of the motivation that underlies in-group bias. Psychological Science, 25(4), 921–927.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  20. Cameron, C. D., Lindquist, K. A., & Gray, K. (2015). A constructionist review of morality and emotions: No evidence for specific links between moral content and discrete emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19(4), 371–394.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  21. Camras, L. A., & Allison, K. (1985). Children’s understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 9(2), 84–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Camras, L. A., Bakeman, R., Chen, Y., Norris, K., & Cain, T. R. (2006). Culture, ethnicity, and children’s facial expressions: A study of European American, mainland Chinese, Chinese American, and adopted Chinese girls. Emotion, 6(1), 103–114.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  23. Cannon, P. R., Schnall, S., & White, M. (2011). Transgressions and expressions: Affective facial muscle activity predicts moral judgments. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(3), 325–331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Carr, D., & Friedman, M. A. (2005). Is obesity stigmatizing? Body weight, perceived discrimination, and psychological well-being in the United States. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 46(3), 244–259.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  25. Case, T. I., Repacholi, B. M., & Stevenson, R. J. (2006). My baby doesn’t smell as bad as yours: The plasticity of disgust. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 357–365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Cashdan, E. (1994). A sensitive period for learning about food. Human Nature, 5(3), 279–291.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  27. Chapman, H. A., & Anderson, A. K. (2012). Understanding disgust. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251(1), 62–76.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2009). In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323(5918), 1222–1226.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  29. Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups: A Sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice”. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 770–789.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  30. Crivelli, C., & Fridlund, A. J. (2018). Facial displays are tools for social influence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(5), 388–399.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Curtis, V. (2011). Why disgust matters. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1583), 3478–3490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Curtis, V. (2013). Don’t look, don’t touch, don’t eat: The science behind revulsion. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Curtis, V., Aunger, R., & Rabie, T. (2004). Evidence that disgust evolved to protect from risk of disease. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271(Suppl_4), S131–S133.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Curtis, V., & Biran, A. (2001). Dirt, disgust, and disease: Is hygiene in our genes? Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44(1), 17–31.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  35. Curtis, V., de Barra, M., & Aunger, R. (2011). Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1583), 389–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Curtis, V. A. (2014). Infection-avoidance behaviour in humans and other animals. Trends in Immunology, 35(10), 457–464.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Curtis, V. A., Danquah, L. O., & Aunger, R. V. (2009). Planned, motivated and habitual hygiene behaviour: An eleven country review. Health Education Research, 24(4), 655–673.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Danovitch, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Children’s extension of disgust to physical and moral events. Emotion, 9(1), 107–112.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1872).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Davey, G. C. L. (2011). Disgust: The disease-avoidance emotion and its dysfunctions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1583), 3453–3465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Davey, G. C. L., Buckland, G., Tantow, B., & Dallos, R. (1998). Disgust and eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 6(3), 201–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. de Barra, M., Islam, M. S., & Curtis, V. (2014). Disgust sensitivity is not associated with health in a rural Bangladeshi sample. PLoS One, 9(6), e100444–e100447.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. de Jong, P. J., Andrea, H., & Muris, P. (1997). Spider phobia in children: Disgust and fear before and after treatment. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 559–562.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  44. de Jong, P. J., & Merckelbach, H. (1998). Blood-injection-injury phobia and fear of spiders: Domain specific individual differences in disgust sensitivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 153–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Deacon, B., & Olatunji, B. O. (2007). Specificity of disgust sensitivity in the prediction of behavioral avoidance in contamination fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(9), 2110–2120.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. DeJesus, J. M., Gerdin, E., Sullivan, K. R., & Kinzler, K. D. (2019). Children judge others based on their food choices. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 143–161.Google Scholar
  47. DeJesus, J. M., Shutts, K., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). Eww she sneezed! Contamination context affects children’s food preferences and consumption. Appetite, 87, 303–309.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Douglas, M. (1966). Purity and danger. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
  49. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1976). Measuring facial movement. Environmental Psychology and Nonverbal Behavior, 1(1), 56–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotion. Science, 164(3875), 86–88.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  52. Fallon, A. E., Rozin, P., & Pliner, P. (1984). The child’s conception of food: The development of food rejections with special reference to disgust and contamination sensitivity. Child Development, 55, 566–575.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  53. Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 7(4), 333–353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Feder, Y. (2015). Contamination appraisals, pollution beliefs, and the role of cultural inheritance in shaping disease avoidance behavior. Cognitive Science, 40(6), 1561–1585.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  55. Fessler, D. M. T., & Navarrete, C. D. (2003). Meat is good to taboo: Dietary proscriptions as a product of the interaction of psychological mechanisms and social processes. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 3(1), 1–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Fincher, C. L., & Thornhill, R. (2012). Parasite-stress promotes in-group assortative sociality: The cases of strong family ties and heightened religiosity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 61–78.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  57. Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1995). DSM-IV field trial: Obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 90–96.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  58. Freud, S. (2017). Three essays on the theory of sexuality: The 1905 edition. (U. Kistner, Trans.). London, UK: Verso. (Original work published 1905)Google Scholar
  59. Gagnon, M., Gosselin, P., Hudon-ven Der Buhs, I., Larocque, K., & Milliard, K. (2010). Children’s recognition and discrimination of fear and disgust facial expressions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34(1), 27–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Galef, B. G. (1989). Enduring social enhancement of rats’ preferences for the palatable and the piquant. Appetite, 13, 81–92.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  61. Gere, A., Székely, G., Kovács, S., Kókai, Z., & Sipos, L. (2017). Readiness to adopt insects in Hungary: A case study. Food Quality and Preference, 59, 81–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Gilbert, A. N., Fridlund, A. J., & Sabini, J. (1987). Hedonic and social determinants of facial displays to odors. Chemical Senses, 12(2), 355–363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chapman, H. A. (2017). Beyond purity. Psychological Science, 28(1), 80–91.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  64. Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Kluck, B., & Cornwell, R. (2001). I am not an animal: Mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human creatureliness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 427–435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Grill, H. J., & Norgren, R. (1978). The taste reactivity test. II. Mimetic responses to gustatory stimuli in chronic thalamic and chronic decerebrate rats. Brain Research, 143(2), 281–297.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  66. Haidt, J., McCauley, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: A scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Personality and Individual Differences, 16(5), 701–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Haidt, J., Rozin, P., McCauley, C., & Imada, S. (1997). Body, psyche, and culture: The relationship between disgust and morality. Psychology and Developing Societies, 9(1), 107–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557–560.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  69. Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 847–853.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  70. Harris, M. (1985). Good to eat: Riddles of food and culture. Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press.Google Scholar
  71. Hart, B. L. (1990). Behavioral adaptations to pathogens and parasites: Five strategies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 14(3), 273–294.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  72. Hartmann, C., Shi, J., Giusto, A., & Siegrist, M. (2015). The psychology of eating insects: A cross-cultural comparison between Germany and China. Food Quality and Preference, 44, 148–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Harvey, T., Troop, N. A., Treasure, J. I., & Murphy, T. (2002). Fear, disgust, and abnormal eating attitudes: A preliminary study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32(2), 213–218.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  74. Hayes, C. J., Stevenson, R. J., & Coltheart, M. (2007). Disgust and Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 45(6), 1135–1151.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  75. Hejmadi, A., Rozin, P., & Siegal, M. (2004). Once in contact, always in contact: Contagious essence and conceptions of purification in American and Hindu Indian children. Developmental Psychology, 40(4), 467–476.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  76. Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2010). The evolution of cultural adaptations: Fijian food taboos protect against dangerous marine toxins. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1701), 3715–3724.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  77. Herz, R. (2012). That’s disgusting: Unraveling the mysteries of repulsion. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
  78. Hodson, G., Choma, B. L., Boisvert, J., Hafer, C. L., MacInnis, C. C., & Costello, K. (2013). The role of intergroup disgust in predicting negative outgroup evaluations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(2), 195–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Hodson, G., & Costello, K. (2007). Interpersonal disgust, ideological orientations, and dehumanization as predictors of intergroup attitudes. Psychological Science, 18(8), 691–698.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  80. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., Iyer, R., & Haidt, J. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(5), 537–544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., & Bloom, P. (2009). Conservatives are more easily disgusted than liberals. Cognition & Emotion, 23(4), 714–725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence from developmental and cross-cultural research. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 288–299.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  83. Izard, C. E., Huebner, R. R., Risser, D., & Dougherty, L. (1980). The young infant’s ability to produce discrete emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 16(2), 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Jabbi, M., Bastiaansen, J., & Keysers, C. (2008). A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity pathways. PLoS One, 3(8), e2939.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Jäncke, L., & Kaufmann, N. (1994). Facial EMG responses to odors in solitude and with an audience. Chemical Senses, 19(2), 99–111.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  86. Kalish, C. W. (1998). Young children’s predictions of illness: Failure to recognize probabilistic causation. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 1046–1058.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  87. Kalyva, E., Pellizzoni, S., Tavano, A., Iannello, P., & Siegal, M. (2010). Contamination sensitivity in autism, Down syndrome, and typical development. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4(1), 43–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Kauer, J., Pelchat, M. L., Rozin, P., & Zickgraf, H. F. (2015). Adult picky eating: Phenomenology, taste sensitivity, and psychological correlates. Appetite, 90, 219–228.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  89. Keeler, R. F., & Tu, A. T. (1991). Handbook of natural toxins: Toxicology of plant and fungal compounds (Vol. 6). New York, NY: Marcel Dekker Inc.Google Scholar
  90. Kelly, D. (2011). Yuck! The nature and moral significance of disgust. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Kiesecker, J. M., Skelly, D. K., Beard, K. H., & Preisser, E. (1999). Behavioral reduction of infection risk. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 96(16), 9165–9168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Knutson, K. L., Spiegel, K., Penev, P., & Van Cauter, E. (2007). The metabolic consequences of sleep deprivation. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 11(3), 163–178.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Kochanska, G., Tjebkes, T. L., & Forman, D. R. (1998). Children’s emerging regulation of conduct: Restraint, compliance, and internalization from infancy to the second year. Child Development, 69(5), 1378–1389.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  94. Kollareth, D., & Russell, J. A. (2017). Is it disgusting to be reminded that you are an animal? Cognition & Emotion, 31(7), 1318–1332.Google Scholar
  95. Kolnai, A. (2004). On disgust. (B. Smith & C. Korsmeyer, Eds.). Peru, IL: Open Court. (Original work published 1929)Google Scholar
  96. Kupfer, T. R., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2017). Communicating moral motives: The social signaling function of disgust. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(6), 632–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Kurzban, R., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 187–208.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  98. Lafraire, J., Rioux, C., Giboreau, A., & Picard, D. (2016). Food rejections in children: Cognitive and social/environmental factors involved in food neophobia and picky/fussy eating behavior. Appetite, 96(C), 347–357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Landy, J. F., & Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Does incidental disgust amplify moral judgment? A meta-analytic review of experimental evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(4), 518–536.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  100. Legare, C. H., Evans, E. M., Rosengren, K. S., & Harris, P. L. (2012). The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. Child Development, 83(3), 779–793.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  101. Legare, C. H., & Gelman, S. A. (2008). Bewitchment, biology, or both: The co-existence of natural and supernatural explanatory frameworks across development. Cognitive Science, 32(4), 607–642.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  102. Legare, C. H., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). South African children’s understanding of AIDS and flu: Investigating conceptual understanding of cause, treatment and prevention. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 9(3), 333–346.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  103. Legare, C. H., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (2009). Evidence for an explanation advantage in naïve biological reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 58(2), 177–194.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  104. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., & Vasen, A. (1987). Making faces: Age and emotion differences in the posing of emotional expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 690–697.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Liberman, Z., Kinzler, K. D., & Woodward, A. L. (2014). Friends or foes: Infants use shared evaluations to infer others’ social relationships. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 966–971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Sullivan, K. R., & Kinzler, K. D. (2016). Early emerging system for reasoning about the social nature of food. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(34), 9480–9485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121–143.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  108. Lumeng, J. C., Cardinal, T. M., Jankowski, M., Kaciroti, N., & Gelman, S. A. (2008). Children’s use of adult testimony to guide food selection. Appetite, 51(2), 302–310.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Lumeng, J. C., Miller, A., Peterson, K. E., Kaciroti, N., Sturza, J., Rosenblum, K., & Vazquez, D. M. (2014). Diurnal cortisol pattern, eating behaviors and overweight in low-income preschool-aged children. Appetite, 73, 65–72.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  110. Lumley, M. A., & Melamed, B. G. (1992). Blood phobics and nonphobics: Psychological differences and affect during exposure. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 30(5), 425–434.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  111. Malterud, K., & Ulriksen, K. (2011). Obesity, stigma, and responsibility in health care: A synthesis of qualitative studies. International Journal of Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 6(4), 8404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Mancini, G., Agnoli, S., Baldaro, B., Ricci Bitti, P. E., & Surcinelli, P. (2013). Facial expressions of emotions: Recognition accuracy and affective reactions during late childhood. The Journal of Psychology, 147(6), 599–617.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  113. McKay, D., & Tsao, S. D. (2005). A treatment most foul: Handling disgust in cognitive-behavior therapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 19(4), 355–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. McMichael, C., & Robinson, P. (2016). Drivers of sustained hygiene behaviour change: A case study from mid-western Nepal. Social Science & Medicine, 163, 28–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Megido, R. C., Gierts, C., Blecker, C., Brostaux, Y., Haubruge, É., Alabi, T., & Francis, F. (2016). Consumer acceptance of insect-based alternative meat products in Western countries. Food Quality and Preference, 52, 237–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Meissner, K., Muth, E. R., & Herbert, B. M. (2011). Bradygastric activity of the stomach predicts disgust sensitivity and perceived disgust intensity. Biological Psychology, 86(1), 9–16.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  117. Mennella, J. A., Pepino, M. Y., & Reed, D. R. (2005). Genetic and environmental determinants of bitter perception and sweet preferences. Pediatrics, 115(2), e216–e222.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Menninghaus, W. (2003). Disgust: The theory and history of a strong sensation. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  119. Meyer-Rochow, V. (2009). Food taboos: Their origins and purposes. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine, 5(1), 18.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Miller, W. I. (1997). The anatomy of disgust. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  121. Mitchell, I. J., Heims, H., Neville, E. A., & Rickards, H. (2005). Huntington’s disease patients show impaired perception of disgust in the gustatory and olfactory modalities. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 17(1), 119–121.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  122. Moretz, M. W., & McKay, D. (2008). Disgust sensitivity as a predictor of obsessive-compulsive contamination symptoms and associated cognitions. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 707–715.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  123. Moretz, M. W., Rogove, J., & McKay, D. (2011). The role of disgust in childhood anxiety disorders. In D. McKay, E. A. Storch, D. McKay, & E. A. Storch (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent anxiety disorders (pp. 155–170). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Muris, P., Huijding, J., Mayer, B., & de Vries, H. (2012). Does ‘yuck’ mean ‘eek’? Fear responses in children after a disgust manipulation. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychology, 43, 765–769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Muris, P., Huijding, J., Mayer, B., Langkamp, M., Reyhan, E., & Olatunji, B. (2012). Assessment of disgust sensitivity in children with an age-downward version of the Disgust Emotion Scale. Behavior Therapy, 43(4), 876–886.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  126. Muris, P., Mayer, B., Borth, M., & Vos, M. (2013). Nonverbal and verbal transmission of disgust from mothers to offspring: Effects on children’s evaluation of a novel animal. Behavior Therapy, 44(2), 293–301.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  127. Muris, P., Mayer, B., Huijding, J., & Konings, T. (2008). A dirty animal is a scary animal! Effects of disgust-related information on fear beliefs in children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(1), 137–144.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  128. Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Nederkoorn, S., Rassin, E., Candel, I., & Horselenberg, R. (2000). Disgust and psychopathological symptoms in a nonclinical sample. Personality and Individuals Differences, 29, 1163–1167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Muris, P., van der Heiden, S., & Rassin, E. (2008). Disgust sensitivity and psychopathological symptoms in non-clinical children. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 39(2), 133–146.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  130. Nabi, R. L. (2002). The theoretical versus the lay meaning of disgust: Implications for emotion research. Cognition & Emotion, 16(5), 695–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Navarrete, C. D., & Fessler, D. M. T. (2006). Disease avoidance and ethnocentrism: The effects of disease vulnerability and disgust sensitivity on intergroup attitudes. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 270–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Nemeroff, C., & Rozin, P. (1994). The contagion concept in adult thinking in the United States: Transmission of germs and of interpersonal influence. Ethos, 22(2), 158–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Neziroglu, F., Hickey, M., & McKay, D. (2010). Psychophysiological and self-report components of disgust in body dysmorphic disorder: The effects of repeated exposure. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3(1), 40–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Nussbaum, M. (2004). Hiding from humanity: Disgust, shame, and the law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  135. Oar, E. L., Farrell, L. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (2015). One session treatment for specific phobias: An adaptation for paediatric blood–injection–injury phobia in youth. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 18(4), 370–394.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  136. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 303–321.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  137. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2011). Disease avoidance as a functional basis for stigmatization. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1583), 3433–3452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., Wagland, P., Case, T. I., & Repacholi, B. M. (2014). Parent-child transmission of disgust and hand hygiene: The role of vocalizations, gestures and other parental responses. The Psychological Record, 64(4), 803–811.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Olatunji, B. O., Lohr, J. M., Sawchuk, C. N., & Patten, K. (2007). Fear and disgust responding to heterogeneous blood-injection-injury stimuli: Distinctions from anxiety symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29(1), 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Olatunji, B. O., & McKay, D. (Eds.). (2009). Disgust and its disorders: Theory, assessment, and treatment implications. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  141. Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Lohr, J. M., & Sawchuk, C. N. (2005). The structure of disgust: Domain specificity in relation to contamination ideation and excessive washing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(8), 1069–1086.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  142. Olatunji, B. O., Williams, N. L., Tolin, D. F., Abramowitz, J. S., Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., & Elwood, L. S. (2007). The Disgust Scale: Item analysis, factor structure, and suggestions for refinement. Psychological Assessment, 19(3), 281–297.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  143. Page, A. C. (2003). The role of disgust in faintness elicited by blood and injection stimuli. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 45–58.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  144. Park, J. H., Schaller, M., & Crandall, C. S. (2007). Pathogen-avoidance mechanisms and the stigmatization of obese people. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(6), 410–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Peryam, D. R. (1963). The acceptance of novel foods. Food Technology, 17, 33–39.Google Scholar
  146. Petersen, M. B. (2017). Healthy out-group members are represented psychologically as infected in-group members. Psychological Science, 28(12), 1857–1863.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  147. Phelan, S. M., Burgess, D. J., Yeazel, M. W., Hellerstedt, W. L., Griffin, J. M., & Ryn, V. M. (2015). Impact of weight bias and stigma on quality of care and outcomes for patients with obesity. Obesity Reviews, 16(4), 319–326.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Phillips, M. L., Senior, C., Fahy, T., & David, A. S. (1998). Disgust: The forgotten emotion of psychiatry. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 172, 373–375.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  149. Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., Calder, A. J., … David, A. S. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389(6650), 495–498.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  150. Prokop, P., & Fančovičová, J. (2016). Mothers are less disgust sensitive than childless females. Personality and Individual Differences, 96, 65–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  151. Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity. Obesity Research, 9(12), 788–805.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  152. Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2010). Obesity stigma: Important considerations for public health. American Journal of Public Health, 100(6), 1019–1028.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Rakison, D. H., & Oakes, L. M. (Eds.). (2003). Early category and concept development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  154. Raman, L., & Gelman, S. A. (2008). Do children endorse psychosocial factors in the transmission of illness and disgust? Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 801–813.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  155. Raudenbush, B., & Capiola, A. (2012). Physiological responses of food neophobics and food neophilics to food and non-food stimuli. Appetite, 58(3), 1106–1108.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  156. Reed, D. R., & Knaapila, A. (2010). Genetics of taste and smell: Poisons and pleasures. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science, 94, 213–240.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  157. Reicher, S. D., Templeton, A., Neville, F., Ferrari, L., & Drury, J. (2016). Core disgust is attenuated by ingroup relations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(10), 2631–2635.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  158. Reisenzein, R., Studtmann, M., & Horstmann, G. (2013). Coherence between emotion and facial expression: Evidence from laboratory experiments. Emotion Review, 5(1), 16–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  159. Rodger, H., Vizioli, L., Ouyang, X., & Caldara, R. (2015). Mapping the development of facial expression recognition. Developmental Science, 18(6), 926–939.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  160. Rosenstein, D., & Oster, H. (1988). Differential facial responses to four basic tastes in newborns. Child Development, 59(6), 1555–1568.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  161. Rottman, J. (2014). Evolution, development, and the emergence of disgust. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(2), 417–433.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  162. Rottman, J., DeJesus, J. M., & Gerdin, E. (2018). The social origins of disgust. In N. Strohminger & V. Kumar (Eds.), The moral psychology of disgust (pp. 27–52). London, UK: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
  163. Rottman, J., Johnston, A. M., Bierhoff, S., Pelletier, T., Grigoreva, A. D., & Benitez, J. (2019). In sickness and in filth: Developing a disdain for dirty people. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  164. Rottman, J., & Kelemen, D. (2012). Aliens behaving badly: Children’s acquisition of novel purity-based morals. Cognition, 124(3), 356–360.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  165. Rottman, J., Kelemen, D., & Young, L. (2014). Tainting the soul: Purity concerns predict moral judgments of suicide. Cognition, 130(2), 217–226.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Rottman, J., Young, L., & Kelemen, D. (2017). The impact of testimony on children’s moralization of novel actions. Emotion, 17(5), 811–827.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  167. Royzman, E. B., Atanasov, P., Landy, J. F., Parks, A., & Gepty, A. (2014). CAD or MAD? Anger (not disgust) as the predominant response to pathogen-free violations of the divinity code. Emotion, 14(5), 892–907.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  168. Royzman, E. B., & Sabini, J. (2001). Something it takes to be an emotion: The interesting case of disgust. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 31(1), 29–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  169. Rozin, P. (1990). Development in the food domain. Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 555–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  170. Rozin, P. (2008). Hedonic “adaptation”: Specific habituation to disgust/death elicitors as a result of dissecting a cadaver. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(2), 191–194.Google Scholar
  171. Rozin, P., Fallon, A., & Augustoni-Ziskind, M. (1985). The child’s conception of food: The development of contamination sensitivity to ‘disgusting’ substances. Developmental Psychology, 21(6), 1075–1079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  172. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 23–41.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  173. Rozin, P., Haddad, B., Nemeroff, C., & Slovic, P. (2015). Psychological aspects of the rejection of recycled water: Contamination, purification and disgust. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(1), 50–63.Google Scholar
  174. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & Fincher, K. (2009). From oral to moral. Science, 323, 1179–1180.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  175. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2016). Disgust. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 815–834). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  176. Rozin, P., Hammer, L., Oster, H., Horowitz, T., & Marmora, V. (1986). The child’s conception of food: Differentiation of categories of rejected substances in the 16 months to 5 year age range. Appetite, 7(2), 141–151.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  177. Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 574–586.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  178. Rozin, P., Nemeroff, C., Horowitz, M., Gordon, B., & Voet, W. (1995). The borders of the self: Contamination sensitivity and potency of the body apertures and other body parts. Journal of Research in Personality, 29(3), 318–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  179. Rozin, P., & Schiller, D. (1980). The nature and acquisition of a preference for chili pepper by humans. Motivation and Emotion, 4(1), 77–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  180. Ruba, A. L., Johnson, K. M., Harris, L. T., & Wilbourn, M. P. (2017). Developmental changes in infants’ categorization of anger and disgust facial expressions. Developmental Psychology, 53(10), 1826–1832.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  181. Ruby, M. B., Rozin, P., & Chan, C. (2015). Determinants of willingness to eat insects in the USA and India. Journal of Insects as Food and Feed, 1(3), 215–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  182. Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? A review of the cross-cultural studies. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 102–141.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  183. Russell, P. S., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2013). Bodily moral disgust: What it is, how it is different from anger, and why it is an unreasoned emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 328–351.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  184. Sandberg, H. (2007). A matter of looks: The framing of obesity in four Swedish daily newspapers. Communications, 32, 447–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  185. Sawchuk, C. N. (2009). The acquisition and maintenance of disgust: Developmental and learning perspectives. In B. O. Olatunji & D. McKay (Eds.), Disgust and its disorders: Theory, assessment, and treatment implications (pp. 77–97). Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  186. Sawchuk, C. N., Lohr, J. M., Westendorf, D. H., Meunier, S. A., & Tolin, D. F. (2002). Emotional responding to fearful and disgusting stimuli in specific phobics. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(9), 1031–1046.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  187. Schaich Borg, J., Lieberman, D., & Kiehl, K. A. (2008). Infection, incest, and iniquity: Investigating the neural correlates of disgust and morality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(9), 1529–1546.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  188. Schaller, M. (2011). The behavioural immune system and the psychology of human sociality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 366(1583), 3418–3426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  189. Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(2), 99–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  190. Schienle, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., Blecker, C., Ott, U., Kirsch, P., … Vaitl, D. (2002). The insula is not specifically involved in disgust processing: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 13(16), 2023–2026.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  191. Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1096–1109.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  192. Schwartz, M. B., Chambliss, H. O. N., Brownell, K. D., Blair, S. N., & Billington, C. (2003). Weight bias among health professionals specializing in obesity. Obesity Research, 11(9), 1033–1039.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  193. Shenhav, A., & Mendes, W. B. (2014). Aiming for the stomach and hitting the heart: Dissociable triggers and sources for disgust reactions. Emotion, 14(2), 301–309.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  194. Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., & DeJesus, J. M. (2013). Understanding infants’ and children’s social learning about foods: Previous research and new prospects. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 419–425.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  195. Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., McKee, C. B., & Spelke, E. S. (2009). Social information guides infants’ selection of foods. Journal of Cognition and Development, 10(1–2), 1–17.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  196. Siegal, M., Fadda, R., & Overton, P. G. (2011). Contamination sensitivity and the development of disease-avoidant behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1583), 3427–3432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  197. Siegal, M., & Share, D. L. (1990). Contamination sensitivity in young children. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 455–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  198. Simpson, J., Carter, S., Anthony, S. H., & Overton, P. G. (2006). Is disgust a homogeneous emotion? Motivation and Emotion, 30(1), 31–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  199. Slaughter, V., & Griffiths, M. (2007). Death understanding and fear of death in young children. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 12(4), 525–535.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  200. Smith, D. L. (2011). Less than human: Why we demean, enslave, and exterminate others. New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
  201. Solomon, G. E. A., & Cassimatis, N. L. (1999). On facts and conceptual systems: Young children’s integration of their understandings of germs and contagion. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 113–126.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  202. Soussignan, R., & Schaal, B. (1996). Children’s facial responsiveness to odors: Influences of hedonic valence of odor, gender, age, and social presence. Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 367–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  203. Soussignan, R., Schaal, B., Marlier, L., & Jiang, T. (1997). Facial and autonomic responses to biological and artificial olfactory stimuli in human neonates: Re-examining early hedonic discrimination of odors. Physiology & Behavior, 62(4), 745–758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  204. Speece, M. W., & Brent, S. B. (1984). Children’s understanding of death: A review of three components of a death concept. Child Development, 55, 1671–1686.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  205. Speltini, G., & Passini, S. (2014). Cleanliness/dirtiness, purity/impurity as social and psychological issues. Culture & Psychology, 20(2), 203–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  206. Speth, J. D. (2017). Putrid meat and fish in the Eurasian Middle and Upper Paleolithic: Are we missing a key part of Neanderthal and modern human diet? PaleoAnthropology, 2017, 44–72.Google Scholar
  207. Stark, R., Walter, B., Schienle, A., & Vaitl, D. (2005). Psychophysiological correlates of disgust and disgust sensitivity. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19(1), 50–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  208. Steckler, C. M., Liberman, Z., Van de Vondervoort, J. W., Slevinsky, J., Le, D. T., & Hamlin, J. K. (2018). Feeling out a link between feeling and infant sociomoral evaluation. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(3), 482–500.Google Scholar
  209. Steiner, J. E., Glaser, D., Hawilo, M. E., & Berridge, K. C. (2001). Comparative expression of hedonic impact: Affective reactions to taste by human infants and other primates. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(1), 53–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  210. Stevenson, R., Oaten, M., Case, T., & Repacholi, B. (2014). Is disgust prepared? A preliminary examination in young children. The Journal of General Psychology, 141(4), 326–347.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  211. Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I., & Oaten, M. J. (2009). Frequency and recency of infection and their relationship with disgust and contamination sensitivity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(5), 363–368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  212. Stevenson, R. J., Oaten, M. J., Case, T. I., Repacholi, B. M., & Wagland, P. (2010). Children’s response to adult disgust elicitors: Development and acquisition. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 165–177.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  213. Strauss, R. S., & Pollack, H. A. (2003). Social marginalization of overweight children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 157(8), 746–752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  214. Strohminger, N. (2014). Disgust talked about. Philosophy Compass, 9(7), 478–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  215. Strohminger, N., & Kumar, V. (Eds.). (2018). The moral psychology of disgust. London, UK: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
  216. Thorpe, S. J., Patel, S. P., & Simonds, L. M. (2003). The relationship between disgust sensitivity, anxiety, and obsessions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1397–1409.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  217. Tolin, D. F., Lohr, J. M., Sawchuk, C. N., & Lee, T. C. (1997). Disgust and disgust sensitivity in blood- injection-injury and spider phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 949–953.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  218. Tolin, D. F., Worhunsky, P., & Maltby, N. (2004). Sympathetic magic in contamination- related OCD. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 35, 193–205.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  219. Toyama, N. (2016). Adults’ explanations and children’s understanding of contagious illnesses, non-contagious illnesses, and injuries. Early Child Development and Care, 186(4), 526–543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  220. Tsao, S. D., & McKay, D. (2004). Behavioral avoidance tests and disgust in contamination fears: Distinctions from trait anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(2), 207–216.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  221. Tybur, J. M., & Lieberman, D. (2016). Human pathogen avoidance adaptations. Current Opinion in Psychology, 7, 6–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  222. Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., & Griskevicius, V. (2009). Microbes, mating, and morality: Individual differences in three functional domains of disgust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 103–122.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  223. Tybur, J. M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R., & DeScioli, P. (2013). Disgust: Evolved function and structure. Psychological Review, 120(1), 65–84.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  224. Van de Vondervoort, J. W., & Hamlin, J. K. (2018). The early emergence of sociomoral evaluation: Infants prefer prosocial others. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 77–81.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  225. van Dongen, E. (2001). It isn’t something to yodel about, but it exists! Faeces, nurses, social relations and status within a mental hospital. Aging & Mental Health, 5(3), 205–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  226. van Huis, A., Van Itterbeeck, J., Klunder, H., Mertens, E., Halloran, A., Muir, G., & Vantomme, P. (2013). Edible insects: Future prospects for food and feed security. Rome, Italy: FAO.Google Scholar
  227. Van Overveld, W. M., de Jong, P. J., Peters, M. L., Cavanagh, K., & Davey, G. L. (2006). Disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: Separate constructs that are differentially related to specific fears. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(7), 1241–1252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  228. Ventura, A. K., & Mennella, J. A. (2011). Innate and learned preferences for sweet taste during childhood. Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition & Metabolic Care, 14(4), 379–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  229. Vernon, L. L., & Berenbaum, H. (2002). Disgust and fear in response to spiders. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 809–830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  230. Viar-Paxton, M. A., Ebesutani, C., Kim, E. H., Ollendick, T., Young, J., & Olatunji, B. O. (2015). Development and initial validation of the Child Disgust Scale. Psychological Assessment, 27(3), 1082–1096.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  231. Vytal, K., & Hamann, S. (2010). Neuroimaging support for discrete neural correlates of basic emotions: A voxel-based meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2864–2885.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  232. Wagemans, F. M. A., Brandt, M. J., & Zeelenberg, M. (2018). Disgust sensitivity is primarily associated with purity-based moral judgments. Emotion, 18(2), 277–289.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  233. Wardle, J., Guthrie, C. A., Sanderson, S., & Rapoport, L. (2001). Development of the children’s eating behaviour questionnaire. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(7), 963–970.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  234. Wertz, A. E., & Wynn, K. (2014a). Selective social learning of plant edibility in 6- and 18-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 25(4), 874–882.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  235. Wertz, A. E., & Wynn, K. (2014b). Thyme to touch: Infants possess strategies that protect them from dangers posed by plants. Cognition, 130(1), 44–49.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  236. Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16(10), 780–784.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  237. Widen, S. C., & Naab, P. (2012). Can an anger face also be scared? Malleability of facial expressions. Emotion, 12(5), 919–925.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  238. Widen, S. C., Pochedly, J. T., Pieloch, K., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Introducing the sick face. Motivation and Emotion, 37(3), 550–557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  239. Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2008). Children’s and adults’ understanding of the ‘disgust face’. Cognition & Emotion, 22(8), 1513–1541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  240. Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2013). Children’s recognition of disgust in others. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 271–299.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  241. Wilson, R. R. (2002). The hydra’s tale: Imagining disgust. Edmonton, Alberta: The University of Alberta Press.Google Scholar
  242. Wisneski, D. C., & Skitka, L. J. (2017). Moralization through moral shock: Exploring emotional antecedents to moral conviction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(2), 139–150.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  243. Yoder, A. M., Widen, S. C., & Russell, J. A. (2016). The word disgust may refer to more than one emotion. Emotion, 16(3), 301–308.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  244. Zickgraf, H. F., Franklin, M. E., & Rozin, P. (2016). Adult picky eaters with symptoms of Avoidant/Restrictive Food Intake Disorder: Comparable distress and comorbidity but different eating behaviors compared to those with disordered eating symptoms. Journal of Eating Disorders, 4(1), 26–37.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Joshua Rottman
    • 1
    Email author
  • Jasmine DeJesus
    • 2
  • Heather Greenebaum
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyFranklin and Marshall CollegeLancasterUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of North Carolina at GreensboroGreensboroUSA
  3. 3.School of Social Policy and PracticeUniversity of PennsylvaniaPhiladelphiaUSA

Personalised recommendations