Advertisement

Enhancing Young Brains in Contemporary Family Life: Exploring the Context and Scope of Pediatric Neuro-enhancement

  • Cliodhna O’ConnorEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Advances in Neuroethics book series (AIN)

Abstract

This chapter argues that pediatric neuro-enhancement decisions cannot meaningfully be abstracted from their contextualization within contemporary cultures of parenting, which are inextricably connected with dynamics of nationality, gender, and class. Thus far, most empirical investigation of pediatric neuro-enhancement has focused on how parents of children with neurocognitive disorders engage with enhancement technologies. Very little research explores how people might seek to enhance healthy children’s brains by adapting daily decisions about nutrition, leisure, and education routines. This chapter reviews the current state of knowledge regarding the day-to-day realities of such efforts to enhance children’s brains. It pays particular attention to two empirical studies that shed some light on everyday manifestations of pediatric neuro-enhancement ideas: a study of British press coverage of brain research, and interviews with members of the British public. Newspaper analysis shows that pediatric neuro-enhancement receives considerable media coverage and that this content reproduces cultural ideals and preconceptions regarding parents and families. However, the extent to which this media discourse resonates in the everyday lives of the lay public remains uncertain. The chapter argues that understanding the promises and perils of emerging neuro-enhancement technologies requires more research illuminating how they might interact with the dynamics of contemporary family life.

References

  1. Bangerter A, Heath C (2004) The Mozart effect: tracking the evolution of a scientific legend. Br J Soc Psychol 43(4):605–623CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bauer MW (2005) Public perceptions and mass media in the biotechnology controversy. Int J Public Opin Res 17:5–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brosnan C (2011) The sociology of neuroethics: expectational discourses and the rise of a new discipline. Sociol Compass 5(4):287–297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cabrera L, Fitz NS, Reiner PB (2015) Empirical support for the moral salience of the therapy-enhancement distinction in the debate over cognitive, affective and social enhancement. Neuroethics 8(3):243–256CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cameron D (2016) Prime Minister’s speech on life chances. https://www.gov.uk/government/speeches/prime-ministers-speech-on-life-chances. Accessed 13 Dec 2017
  6. Caputo V (2007) She’s from a ‘good family’. Childhood 14(2):173–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Choudhury S, McKinney KA, Merten M (2012) Rebelling against the brain: Public engagement with the “neurological adolescent”. Soc Sci Med 74(4):565–573CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Condit CM (2011) When do people deploy genetic determinism? A review pointing to the need for multi-factorial theories of public utilization of scientific discourses. Sociol Compass 5:618–635CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Coveney C (2011) Cognitive enhancement: exploring modafinil use in social context. In: Pickersgill M, van Keulen I (eds) Sociological reflections on the neurosciences. Emerald, Bingley, pp 203–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Coveney CM, Nerlich B, Martin P (2009) Modafinil in the media: metaphors, medicalisation and the body. Soc Sci Med 68(3):487–495CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  11. Faber NS, Savulescu J, Douglas T (2016) Why is cognitive enhancement deemed unacceptable? The role of fairness, deservingness, and hollow achievements. Front Psychol 7:232.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00232CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  12. Farah MJ, Illes J, Cook-Deegan R, Gardner H, Kandel E, King P et al (2004) Neurocognitive enhancement: what can we do and what should we do? Nat Rev Neurosci 5(5):421–425CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Farr RM (1993) Common sense, science and social representations. Public Underst Sci 2:189–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fitz NS, Nadler R, Manogaran P, Chong EW, Reiner PB (2014) Public attitudes toward cognitive enhancement. Neuroethics 7(2):173–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Forlini C, Hall W (2016) The is and ought of the ethics of neuroenhancement: mind the gap. Front Psychol 6:1998.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01998CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  16. Forlini C, Racine E (2009) Disagreements with implications: diverging discourses on the ethics of non-medical use of methylphenidate for performance enhancement. BMC Med Ethics 10(1):9CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Franke AG, Bagusat C, Rus S, Engel A, Lieb K (2014) Substances used and prevalence rates of pharmacological cognitive enhancement among healthy subjects. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 264(1):83–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Haraway D (1997) Modest_Witness@Second_Millennium.FemaleMan _Meets_OncoMouseTM: Feminism and Technoscience. Routledge, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  19. Hays S (1996) The cultural contradictions of motherhood. Yale University Press, New HavenGoogle Scholar
  20. Joffe H, Elsey JW (2014) Free association in psychology and the grid elaboration method. Rev Gen Psychol 18(3):173CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lareau A (2002) Invisible inequality: social class and childrearing in black families and white families. Am Sociol Rev 67(5):747–776CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lee EJ (2008) Living with risk in the age of “intensive motherhood”: maternal identity and infant feeding. Health Risk Soc 10(5):467–477CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Macvarish J, Lee E, Lowe P (2014) The “first three years” movement and the infant brain: a review of critiques. Sociol Compass 8(6):792–804CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Maxwell B, Racine E (2012) Does the neuroscience research on early stress justify responsive childcare? Examining interwoven epistemological and ethical challenges. Neuroethics 5(2):159–172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Munro E, Musholt K (2014) Neuroscience and the risks of maltreatment. Child Youth Serv Rev 47(1):18–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Nadesan MH (2002) Engineering the entrepreneurial infant: brain science, infant development toys, and governmentality. Cult Stud 16(3):401–432CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. O’Connell G, De Wilde J, Haley J, Shuler K, Schafer B, Sandercock P, Wardlaw JM (2011) The brain, the science and the media. EMBO Rep 12(7):630–636CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. O’Connor C (2016) Science in society: neuroscience and lay understandings of self and identity. In: Absher JR, Cloutier J (eds) Neuroimaging personality, social cognition and character: traits and mental states in the brain. Elsevier, London, pp 371–382CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. O’Connor C, Joffe H (2013a) Media representations of early human development: protecting, feeding and loving the developing brain. Soc Sci Med 97:297–306CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. O’Connor C, Joffe H (2013b) How has neuroscience affected lay understandings of personhood? A review of the evidence. Public Underst Sci 22(3):254–268CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. O’Connor C, Joffe H (2014) Social representations of brain research: exploring public (dis)engagement with contemporary neuroscience. Sci Commun 36(5):617–645CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. O’Connor C, Joffe H (2015) How the public engages with brain optimization: the media-mind relationship. Sci Tech Hum Value 40(5):712–743CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. O’Connor C, Rees G, Joffe H (2012) Neuroscience in the public sphere. Neuron 74(2):220–226CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Parens E (2002) How far will the treatment/enhancement distinction get us as we grapple with new ways to shape our selves? In: Marcus SJ (ed) Neuroethics: mapping the field. The DANA Press, New York, pp 152–158Google Scholar
  35. Partridge BJ, Bell SK, Lucke JC, Yeates S, Hall WD (2011) Smart drugs “as common as coffee”: media hype about neuroenhancement. PLoS One 6(11):e28416CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Pickersgill M (2013) The social life of the brain: neuroscience in society. Curr Sociol 61(3):322–340CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  37. Pickersgill M, Cunningham-Burley S, Martin P (2011) Constituting neurologic subjects: neuroscience, subjectivity and the mundane significance of the brain. Subjectivity 4(3):346–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Pickersgill M, Martin P, Cunningham-Burley S (2015) The changing brain: neuroscience and the enduring import of everyday experience. Public Underst Sci 24(7):878–892CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  39. Pitts-Taylor V (2010) The plastic brain: neoliberalism and the neuronal self. Health 14(6):635–652CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Racine E, Waldman S, Rosenberg J, Illes J (2010) Contemporary neuroscience in the media. Soc Sci Med 71(4):725–733CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  41. Rose N (1996) Inventing our selves: psychology, power, and personhood. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Rose H, Rose S (2016) Can neuroscience change our minds? Polity Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  43. Singh I (2002) Bad boys, good mothers, and the “miracle” of Ritalin. Sci Context 15(4):577–603CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Singh I (2003) Boys will be boys: fathers’ perspectives on ADHD symptoms, diagnosis, and drug treatment. Harv Rev Psychiatry 11(6):308–316PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Singh I (2004) Doing their jobs: mothering with Ritalin in a culture of mother-blame. Soc Sci Med 59(6):1193–1205CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Singh I (2005) Will the “real boy” please behave: dosing dilemmas for parents of boys with ADHD. Am J Bioeth 5(3):34–47CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Singh I (2013) Brain talk: power and negotiation in children’s discourse about self, brain and behaviour. Sociol Health Illn 35(6):813–827CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Singh I, Baker L (2013) ADHD and Stigma: the role of environmental factors. ADHD Pract 5(2):4–7Google Scholar
  49. Singh I, Kelleher KJ (2010) Neuroenhancement in young people: proposal for research, policy, and clinical management. AJOB Neurosci 1(1):3–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ten Eyck TA (2005) The media and public opinion on genetics and biotechnology: mirrors, windows, or walls? Public Underst Sci 14:305–316CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Thompson RA, Nelson CA (2001) Developmental science and the media—early brain development. Am Psychol 56(1):5–15CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Thornton DJ (2011a) Neuroscience, affect, and the entrepreneurialization of motherhood. Commun Crit-Cult Stud 8(4):399–424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Thornton DJ (2011b) Brain culture: neuroscience and popular media. Rutgers University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  54. Wall G (2010) Mothers’ experiences with intensive parenting and brain development discourse. Women’s Stud Int Forum 33(3):253–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Whiteley L (2012) Resisting the revelatory scanner? Critical engagements with fMRI in popular media. BioSocieties 7(3):245–272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rutter M (1998) Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 39(4):465-476CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of PsychologyUniversity College DublinDublinIreland

Personalised recommendations