Skip to main content

Some theoretical Prerequisites for the Integrated Study of Linguistic “Macrochange”

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Complexity Applications in Language and Communication Sciences
  • 320 Accesses

Abstract

The term macrochange (following the example of macroevolution as used in evolutionary theory) is here proposed to characterize changes affecting several areas of a language at the same time with a unitary cause, inside or outside language proper. A general theoretical model for this type of change will be proposed. As a case study, a set of changes in Icelandic will be analysed that lead to the creation of a widespread system for the marking of the agents’ or experiencers’ responsibility, evidentiality , etc., and that affect the lexicon , morphology , syntax and discourse. This macrochange will be defined in the terms of the integrated view of language and language change proposed here, which makes it necessary to adopt and develop the methods and concepts of complexity theory as applied to language . From this perspective, ‘language ’ has to be taken to include not only structures and usage, but the whole ‘ecological niche’ where it exists and is in use by human beings in specific cultural and historical situations; it is to be viewed as a complex natural phenomenon.

Partial support for work on this topic was provided by Research Project FFI2013-41366-P. MINECO. Lingüística cognitiva y lenguaje disfuncional.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The translations are obviously not idiomatic in English: their only purpose is to make possible the literal understanding of the Icelandic examples.

References

  • Aikhenvald, A. Y. (2003). Mechanisms of change in areal diffusion: New morphology and language contact. Journal of Linguistics, 39, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aikhenvald, A. Y., & Dixon, R. M. W. (Eds.). (2001). Areal diffusion and genetic inheritance. Oxford: Oxford U.P.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barðdal, J. (2003). Case and argument structure of novel verbs of communication in Icelandic. In L. O. Delsing, C. Falk, G. Josefsson, & H. Á. Sigurðsson (Eds.), Grammatik i Fokus (Vol. 2, pp. 25–35). Lund: Lund Universitet.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barðdal, J., & Chelliah, S. L. (Eds.). (2009). The role of semantic, pragmatic, and discourse factors in the development of case. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barðdal, J., & Eythórsson, T. (2003). The change that never happened: The story of oblique subjects. Journal of Linguistics, 39, 439–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartmiński, J., & Zinken, J. (Eds.). (2009). Aspects of cognitive ethnolinguistics. (Advances in cognitive linguistics). London: Equinox Publishing Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastardas-Boada, A. (2013). In A. Massip-Bonet & A. Bastardas-Boada (Eds.), Complexity perspectives on language, communication and society (pp. 15–34). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-32817-6_3.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bernárdez, E. (1997). A partial synergetic model of deagentivisation. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 4, 53–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernárdez, E. (2005). Toward a common history of the Germanic and European languages in the Middle Ages. REVISTA SELIM Journal of the Spanish Society for Medieval English Language and Literature, 12, 5–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernárdez, E. (2007a). The unconscious, irresponsible construction in modern Icelandic. In C. S. Butler, R. Hidalgo Downing & J. Lavid (Eds.), Functional perspectives on grammar and discourse (pp. 149–164). Amsterdam: Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/slcs.85.09ber.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bernárdez, E. (2007b). Synergy in the construction of meaning. In M. Fabiszak (Ed.), Language and meaning: Cognitive and functional perspectives (pp. 15–37). Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernárdez, E. (2008). Collective cognition and individual activity: Variation, language, and culture. In R. Dirven, R. Frank, E. Bernárdez, & T. Ziemke (Eds.), Body, language, and mind (Vol. 2, pp. 137–176). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernárdez, E. (2013). Evidentiality and the Epistemic Use of the Icelandic Verbs Sjá and Heyra. A cultural linguistic view. In A. Głaz, D. S. Danaher, & P. Łozowski (Eds.), The linguistic worldview. Ethnolinguistics, cognition, and culture (pp. 415–441). London: Versita.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjørnum, S. (2003). Grønlandsk Grammatik. Nuuk: Atuagkat.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ejsing, J. (2004, November 20). Inuitter mangler ord for nye naturfænomener. Berlingske Tidende. Retrived from https://www.b.dk/nationalt/inuitter-mangler-ord-for-nye-naturfaenomener.

  • Enfield, N. J. (Ed.). (2002). Ethnosyntax. Explorations in grammar and culture. Oxford: Oxford U.P.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enfield, N. J. (2003). Linguistic epidemiology: Semantics and grammar of language contact in mainland Southeast Asia. London: Routledge Curzon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Enfield, N. J. (2014). Natural causes of language. Frames, biases, and cultural transmission. Berlin: Language Science Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Enfield, N. J., & Sidnell, J. (2014). Language presupposes an enchronic infrastructure for social interaction. In D. Dor, C. Knight, & J. Lewis (Eds.), The social origins of language (pp. 92–104). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fagua Rincón, D., & Seifart, F. (2010). Aspectos morfosintácticos del ocaina: rasgos genéticos (familia witoto) e influencias areales. Mundo Amazónico, 1, 215–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortescue, M. (1998). Language relations across the Bering Strait: Reappraising the archaeological and linguistic evidence. London: Cassel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fusaroli, R., & Tylén, K. (2012). Carving language for social coordination. A dynamic approach. Interaction Studies, 13(1), 103–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? TRENDS in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 8–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., & Janssen, M. A. (2005). Computational models of collective behaviour. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 9(9), 424–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haspelmath, M. (1998). How young is standard average European? Language Sciences, 20(3), 271–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haspelmath, M. (2001). The European linguistic area: Standard Average European. In M. Haspelmath, E. König, W. Oesterreicher, & W. Raible (Eds.), Language typology and language universals (pp. 1492–1510). Berlin: de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Haspelmath, M. (2004). On directionality in language change with particular reference to grammaticalization. In O. Fischer, M. Norde, & H. Perridon (Eds.), Up and down the cline—the nature of grammaticalization (pp. 17–44). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, B. (1997). Cognitive foundations of grammar. New York: Oxford U.P.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heine, B., & Kuteva, T. (2003). On contact-induced grammaticalization. Studies in Language, 27(3), 529–572.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krawczak, K. (2007). Meaning as an epiphenomenon of cognition, social interaction and intercognition. In M. Fabiszak (Ed.), Language and meaning. Cognitive and functional perspectives (pp. 187–198). Frankfurt/Main: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kress, B. (1982). Isländische Grammatik. Leipzig: Enzyklopädie.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutschera, U., & Niklas, K. J. (2004). The modern theory of biological evolution: An expanded synthesis. Naturwissenschaften, 91, 255–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Massip-Bonet, À. (2013). Language as a complex adaptive process. Towards an Integrative Linguistics. In Á. Massip-Bonet & A. Bastardas-Boada (Eds.), Complexity Perspectives on Language, Communication and Society (pp. 35–60). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Massip-Bonet, À., & Bastardas-Boada, A. (Eds.). (2013). Complexity Perspectives on Language, Communication and Society. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schieffelin, B. (2002). Marking time: The dichotomizing discourse of multiple temporalities. Current Anthropology, 43, 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schouwstra, M., & de Swart, H. (2014). The semantic origins of word order. Cognition, 131, 431–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Severi, C. (2004). Capturing Imagination: A cognitive approach to cultural complexity. The Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 10(4), 815–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharifian, F. (Ed.). (2015). The Routledge Handbook of Language and Culture. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thráinsson, H. (2007). The Syntax of Icelandic. Cambridge: Cambridge U.P.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Weigand, E. (2010). Linguists and their speakers. Language Sciences, 32, 536–544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Welmers, W. E. (1973). African Language Structures. Berkeley etc.: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, G. (Ed.). (2005). Language, brain, culture. Linguistics and the Human Sciences, 1(2), 147.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, R. A. (2004). Boundaries of the Mind. The individual in the fragile sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge U.P.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Enrique Bernárdez .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Bernárdez, E. (2019). Some theoretical Prerequisites for the Integrated Study of Linguistic “Macrochange”. In: Massip-Bonet, À., Bel-Enguix, G., Bastardas-Boada, A. (eds) Complexity Applications in Language and Communication Sciences. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-04598-2_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-04598-2_6

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-030-04596-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-030-04598-2

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics