Advertisement

Individuals’ Variables in Cognitive Abilities Using a Narrative Serious Game

  • Elena Parra
  • Carla de Juan Ripoll
  • Mariano Alcañiz Raya
  • Irene Alice Chicchi GiglioliEmail author
Conference paper
  • 936 Downloads
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 11243)

Abstract

Age, gender, and education represent crucial variables in the assessment and interpretation of traditional neuropsychological measures as regards the executive functions (EF). Currently, traditional measures are showing limitations in capturing real life behaviors and new technologies, such as serious games, are allowing creating more real situations with higher ecological validity. In the present study, we applied a serious game approach to investigate individual variables-related differences in the EF assessment. 268 healthy subjects participated in the study, completing 14 tasks (6 standard tasks; 8 serious games) randomly presented. The results showed that younger participants completed tasks in less time than older and with higher correct answers. Furthermore, males registered shorter reaction times, while females showed higher percentages of correct answers. The university studies group obtained higher total score and correct answers than high school studies group. Finally, since the study involved technology, we divided the group in high and low use technology level, obtaining that participants with a lower level of use technologies reported higher latency times and lower correct answers in high order EF tasks than the group with higher level of use of technology. As the traditional measure, these findings suggest that individuals’ differences are critical variables to consider in the development of more ecological measures for the assessment of EFs.

Keywords

Serious game Executive functions Age Education Gender Behavioral assessment Ecological validity 

Notes

Funding

This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness funded project “Advanced Therapeutically Tools for Mental Health” (DPI2016-77396-R).

References

  1. 1.
    Stuss, D.T., Alexander, M.P.: Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol. Res. 63(3–4), 289–298 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Diamond, A.: Executive functions. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 64, 135–168 (2013)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Chan, R.C., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., Chen, E.Y.: Assessment of executive functions: review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23(2), 201–216 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Lezak, M.D.: The problem of assessing executive functions. Int. J. Psychol. 17(1–4), 281–297 (1982)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Lezak, M.D., Howieson, D.B., Loring, D.W., Fischer, J.S.: Neuropsychological Assessment. Oxford University Press, Oxford (2004)Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Anderson, P.: Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 8(2), 71–82 (2002)MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. 7.
    Jurado, M.B., Rosselli, M.: The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17(3), 213–233 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Gladsjo, J.A., Schuman, C.C., Evans, J.D., Peavy, G.M., Miller, S.W., Heaton, R.K.: Norms for letter and category fluency: demographic corrections for age, education, and ethnicity. Assessment 6(2), 147–178 (1999)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Stricks, L., Pittman, J., Jacobs, D.M., Sano, M., Stern, Y.: Normative data for a brief neuropsychological battery administered to English-and Spanish-speaking community-dwelling elders. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 4(4), 311–318 (1998)Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Beeri, M.S., et al.: Age, gender, and education norms on the CERAD neuropsychological battery in the oldest old. Neurology 67(6), 1006–1010 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Birch, S.A., Bloom, P.: Understanding children’s and adults’ limitations in mental state reasoning. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8(6), 255–260 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    De Luca, C.R., et al.: Normative data from the CANTAB. I: development of executive function over the lifespan. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25(2), 242–254 (2003)MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Craik, F.I., Salthouse, T.A.: The Handbook of Aging and Cognition. Psychology Press, London (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Ostrosky-Solis, F., Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., Lopez-Arango, G., Uriel-Mendoza, V.: Neuropsychological test performance in illiterate subjects. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 13, 645–660 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Ardila, A., Bertolucci, P.H., Braga, L.W., et al.: Illiteracy: the neuropsychology of cognition without reading. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 25, 689–712 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    Das, S.K., Banerjee, T.K., Mukherjee, C.S., et al.: An urban community-based study of cognitive function among non-demented elderly population in India. Neurol. Asia 11, 37–48 (2006)Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Mathuranath, P.S., Cherian, J.P., Mathew, R., George, A., Alexander, A., Sarma, S.P.: Mini mental state examination and the Addenbrooke’s cognitive examination: effect of education and norms for a multicultural population. Neurol. India 55, 106–110 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Liu, K.P., Kuo, M.C., Tang, K.C., et al.: Effects of age, education and gender in the Consortium to Establish a Registry for the Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD)-Neuropsychological assessment battery for Cantonese-speaking Chinese elders. Int. Psychogeriatr. 23(10), 1575–1581 (2011)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Van Hooren, S.A.H., Valentijn, A.M., Bosma, H., Ponds, R.W.H.M., Van Boxtel, M.P.J., Jolles, J.: Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64–81: a cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 14(1), 40–54 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Messinis, L., Tsakona, I., Malefaki, S., Papathanasopoulos, P.: Normative data and discriminant validity of Rey’s verbal learning test for the Greek adult population. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 22(6), 739–752 (2007)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Proust-Lima, C., Amieva, H., Letenneur, L., Orgogozo, J.M., Jacqmin-Gadda, H., Dartigues, J.F.: Gender and education impact on brain aging: a general cognitive factor approach. Psychol. Aging 23(3), 608 (2008)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Unger, R.K.: Toward a redefinition of sex and gender. Am. Psycholog. 34(11), 1085 (1979)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    Zarghi, A., Zarindast, M.R.: Demographic variables and selective, sustained attention and planning through cognitive tasks among healthy adults. Basic Clin. Neurosci. 2(3), 58–67 (2011)Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Bagherpoor, L.S.K., Akbar, A.: The influence of educational type (virtual and non-virtual) and gender on the executive functions (mental flexibility and selective attention) of students. Bull. Environ. Pharmacol. Life Sci. 2014(3), 360–367 (2014)Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Chaytor, N., Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., Burr, R.: Improving the ecological validity of executive functioning assessment. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 21(3), 217–227 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. 26.
    Spooner, D.M., Pachana, N.A.: Ecological validity in neuropsychological assessment: a case for greater consideration in research with neurologically intact populations. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 21(4), 327–337 (2006)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Burgess, P.W., Alderman, N., Volle, E., Benoit, R.G., Gilbert, S.J.: Mesulam’s frontal lobe mystery re-examined. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 27(5), 493–506 (2009)Google Scholar
  28. 28.
    Chevignard, M., et al.: An ecological approach to planning dysfunction: script execution. Cortex 36(5), 649–669 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Barker, L.A., Andrade, J., Romanowski, C.A.J.: Impaired implicit cognition with intact executive function after extensive bilateral prefrontal pathology: a case study. Neurocase 10(3), 233–248 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Manchester, D., Priestley, N., Jackson, H.: The assessment of executive functions: coming out of the office. Brain Inj. 18(11), 1067–1081 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. 31.
    Fleming, T.M., et al.: Serious games and gamification for mental health: current status and promising directions. Front. Psychiatry 7, 215 (2017)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Andrade, L.H., et al.: Barriers to mental health treatment: results from the WHO World Mental Health surveys. Psychol. Med. 44(6), 1303–1317 (2014)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Fleming, T.M., et al.: Serious games for the treatment or prevention of depression: a systematic review (2014)Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Miller, M.A., Fillmore, M.T.: The effect of word complexity on attentional bias towards alcohol-related words in adult drinkers. Addiction 105, 883–890 (2010)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N.: International affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Technical report A-8. University of Florida, Gainesville (2008)Google Scholar
  36. 36.
    Fillmore, M.T., Rush, C.R., Hays, L.: Acute effects of cocaine in two models of inhibitory control: implications of non-linear dose effects. Addiction 101(132), 3–1332 (2006)Google Scholar
  37. 37.
    Stroop, J.R.: Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 18, 643–662 (1935)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. 38.
    Grant, D.A., Berg, E.: A behavioral analysis of degree of reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-type card-sorting problem. J. Exp. Psychol. 38(4), 404 (1948)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. 39.
    Culberston, W.C., Zillmer, E.A.: Tower of London-Drexel (TOLDX). Examiners’s Manual. Research Version. Multi-Health Systems Inc., Toronto (1999)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Instituto de Investigación e Innovación en Bioingeniería (I3B)Universitat Politècnica de ValènciaValènciaSpain

Personalised recommendations