Skip to main content

The Natural History of Human Language: Bridging the Gaps without Magic

  • Chapter

Abstract

Human languages are quintessentially historical phenomena. Every known aspect of linguistic form and content is subject to change in historical time (Lehmann, 1995; Bybee, 2004). Many facts of language, syntactic no less than semantic, find their explanation in the historical processes that generated them. If adpositions were once verbs, then the fact that they tend to occur on the same side of their arguments as do verbs (“cross-category harmony”: Hawkins, 1983) is a matter of historical contingency rather than a reflection of inherent structural constraints on human language (Delancey, 1993).

Keywords

  • Historical Process
  • Human Language
  • Intergenerational Transmission
  • Humpback Whale
  • Vocal Learning

These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The claim that song is without meaning does not imply that it is without purpose, nor that song characteristics do not convey information. Factors such as proficiency and complexity serve to impress, and different exemplars of song are bound to vary in the extent to which they do so, and thus they do carry information in this sense. But that is something altogether different from the way in which sentence structure is linked to propositional meaning in human language, a feature entirely absent from animal song.

References

  • Alexander, R. D. (1990). How Did Humans Evolve? Reflections on the Uniquely Unique Species. University of Michigan Museum of Zoology Special Publication, 1, 1–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batali, J. (1998). Computational simulations of the emergence of grammar. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy and C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases, pp. 405–426. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batali, J. (2002). The negotiation and acquisition of recursive grammars as a result of competition among exemplars. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic Evolution through Language Acquisition: Formal and Computational Models, pp. 111–172. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Baylis, J. R. (1982). Avian vocal mimicry: Its function and evolution. In D.E. Kroodsma and E.H. Miller (Eds.), Acoustic communication in birds, pp. 51–83. Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, Joan (2004). Mechanisms of change as universals of language. Article to appear in Spanish, English version available for download at http://www.unm.edu/∼jbybee/

  • Catchpole, C. K. (1976). Temporal and sequential organization of song in the sedge warbler (Acrocephalus schoenobaenus). Behaviour, 59, 226–246.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, C. W. and Clapham, P. J. (2004). Acoustic monitoring on a humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) feeding ground shows continual singing into late spring. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 271, 1051–1057.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. D. Appleton & Company, New York.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • DeGusta, D., Gilbert, W. H. and Turner, S. P. (1999). Hypoglossal canal size and hominid speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 96, 1800–1804.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • DeLancey, Scott (1993). Grammaticalization and Linguistic Theory. In J. Gomez de Garcia and D. Rood (Eds.), Proceedings of the 1993 Mid-America Linguistics Conference, pp. 1–22. Department of Linguistics, University of Colorado.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dessalles, J. -L. (1998). Altruism, status, and the origin of relevance. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy and C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language. Social and cognitive bases, pp. 130–148. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devoogd, T. J., Krebs, J. R., Healy, S. D. and Purvis, A. (1993). Relations between song repertoire size and the volume of brain nuclei related to song: comparative evolutionary analysis amongst oscine birds. Proceedings of the Royal Society, London, B254, 75–82.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Devoogd, T. J. 2004. Where is the bird? In H. P. Ziegler and P. Marler (Eds.), The Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong, pp. 778–786. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doupé, A. J. and Kuhl, P. K. (1999). Birdsong and human speech: Common themes and mechanisms. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 22, 567–631.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Enquist, M., Arak, A., Ghirlanda, S. and Wachtmeister, C. -A. (2002). Spectacular phenomena and limits to rationality in genetic and cultural evolution. Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B357, 1585–1594.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J. and Clayton, N.S. (2005). Evolution of the avian brain and intelligence. Current Biology, 15, R946–950.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Farabaugh, S. M. (1982). The ecological and social significance of duetting. In D. E. Kroodsma, E. H. Miller and H. Ouellet (Eds.), Acoustic communication in birds, pp. 84–124. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finlay, B. L. and Darlington, R. B. (1995). Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science, 268, 1578–1584.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2000). The evolution of speech: a comparative review. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 258–267.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch, W. T. (2006). The biology and evolution of music: A comparative perspective. Cognition, 100, 173–215.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Freeberg, T. M. (2000). Culture and courtship in vertebrates: a review of social learning and transmission of courtship systems and mating patterns. Behavioural Processes, 51, 177–192.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55–77.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Geary, D. C. and Flinn, M. V. (2001). Evolution of Human Parental Behavior and the Human Family. Parenting: Science and Practice, 1, 5–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geissmann, T. (2000). Gibbon song and human music from an evolutionary perspective. In NL Wallin, B Merker and S Brown, eds. The origins of music, pp. 103–123. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, M. H., King, A. P. and West, M. J. (2003). Social interaction shapes babbling: testing parallels between birdsong and speech. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 100, 8050–8055.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Haimoff, E. H. (1986). Convergence in the duetting of monogamous Old World primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 15, 51–59.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hasselquist, D., Bensch, S. and von Schantz, T. (1996). Correlation between song repertoire, extra-pair paternity and offspring survival in the great reed warbler. Naure, 381, 229–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkins, J. A. (1983). Word order universals. Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, K. J. and Hayes, C. H. (1951). The intellectual development of a home-raised chimpanzee. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 95, 105–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humboldt, W. von (1836/1988). On language: The diversity of human language structure and its influence on the mental development of mankind. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurford, J. R. (2000). The emergence of syntax. Editorial introduction to the section on syntax in C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy and J. Hurford (Eds.), The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, pp. 219–230. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Hurford, J. (2002) Expression/induction models of language evolution: dimensions and issues. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic Evolution through Language Acquisition: Formal and Computational Models. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iwaniuk, A. N., Dean, K. M. and Nelson J. E. (2005). Interspecific Allometry of the Brain and Brain Regions in Parrots (Psittaciformes): Comparisons with Other Birds and Primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 65, 40–59.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Janik, V. M. and Slater, P. J. B. (1997). Vocal learning in mammals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 26, 59–99.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Jarvis, E. D. (2004). Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language. In H.P. Ziegler and P. Marler (Eds.), Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong, pp. 749–777. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jespersen, O. (1922). Language, its nature, development and origin. London: Allen and Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, F. and Whitney, O. (2005). Singing-driven gene expression in the developing songbird brain. Physiology and Behavior, 86, 390–398.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Jungers, W. L., Pokempner, A. A., Kay, R. F. and Cartmill, M. (2003). Hypoglossal Canal Size in Living Hominoids and the Evolution of Human Speech. Human Biology, 75, 473–184.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Kay, R. F., Cartmill, M. and Balow, M. (1998). The hypoglossal canal and the origin of human vocal behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 5417–5419.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Kellogg, W. N. and Kellogg, L. A. (1933). The Ape and the Child. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, A. P. and West, M. J. (2002). The ontogeny of competence. In D.J. Lewkcowicz and R. Lickliter (Eds.), Conceptions of Development, pp. 77–104. Psychology Press. Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (1998). Language evolution without natural selection: From vocabulary to syntax in a population of learners. Technical Report, Edinburgh Occasional Papers in Linguistics, 98–1, Department of Linguistics, University of Edinburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (2000). Syntax without Natural Selection: How compositionality emerges from vocabulary in a population of learners. In C. Knight, M. Studdert-Kennedy and J. Hurford (Eds.), The Evolutionary Emergence of Language: Social function and the origins of linguistic form, pp. 303–323. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (2001). Spontaneous evolution of linguistic structure: an iterated learning model of the emergence of regularity and irregularity. IEEE Transactions on Evolutionary Computation, 5, 102–110.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, S. (2002) Learning, bottlenecks and the evolution of recursive syntax. In T. Briscoe (Ed.), Linguistic Evolution through Language Acquisition: Formal and Computational Models, pp 173–204. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Knight, C. (1998). Ritual/speech coevolution: a solution tio the problem of deception. In J. R. Hurford, M. Studdert-Kennedy and C. Knight (Eds.), Approaches to the evolution of language: Social and cognitive bases, pp. 68–91. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroodsma, D. E. and Parker, L. D. (1977). Vocal virtuosity in the brown thrasher. Auk, 94, 783–785.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, C. (1995). Thoughts on Grammaticalization. Second, revised edition. München: LINCOM Europa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, P. (1975). On the Origins of Language: An Introduction to the Evolution of Human Speech. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Locke, J. L. and Bogin, B. (in press). Language and life history: A new perspective on the development and evolution of human language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, forthcoming.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, K. (1997). Life history theory and human reproductive behavior: Environmental/contextual influences and heritable variation. Human Nature, 8, 327–359.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • MacLarnon, A. and Hewitt, G. (1999). The evolution of human speech: The role of enhanced breathing control. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 109, 341–363.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P. (1970). Bird song and speech development: could there be parallels? American Scientist, 58, 669–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P. (2000). Origins of music and speech: Insights from animals. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker & S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music, (pp. 31–48). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P. (2004). Bird calls: Their potential for behavioral biology. In HP Ziegler and P Marler, eds. The behavioral neurobiology of birdsong, pp. 31–44. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marler, P., Evans, C. S., and Hauser, M. D. (1992). Animal signals. Reference, motivation or both? In H. Papousek, U. Jürgens, and M. Papousek (Eds.) Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental approaches (pp. 66–86). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merker, B. (2000). Synchronous chorusing and human origins. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker and S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music, pp. 315–327. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merker, B. (2004). Cortex, countercurrent context, and dimensional integration of lifetime memory. Cortex, 40, 559–576.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Merker, B. (2005). The conformal motive in birdsong, music and language. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 17–28.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. F. (1997). Protean primates: The evolution of adaptive unpredictability in competition and courtship. In A. Whiten and R. W. Byrne (Eds.), Machiavellian Intelligence II, pp. 312–340. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: how sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm, F. (1975). A zoologist’s view of some language phenomena, with particular emphasis on vocal learning. In E. H. Lenneberg, & E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Foundations of language development, pp. 61–103. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nottebohm F (1976). Discussion paper. Vocal tract and brain: A search for evolutionary bottlenecks. In S.R. Harnad, H.D. Steklis and J. Lancaster (Eds.), Origins and evolution of language and speech, pp. 643–649. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A. and Peters, S. (2002). Brain development, song learning and mate choice in birds: a review and experimental test of the “nutritional stress hypothesis”. Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Sensory, Neural, and Behavioral Physiology, 188, 1003–1004.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Okanoya, K. (2002). Sexual Display as a Syntactic Vehicle: The Evolution of Syntax in Birdsong and Human Language through Sexual Selection. In A. Wray (Ed.), The transition to language, pp. 44–64. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Payne, K. (2000). The progressively changing songs of humpback whales: A window on the creative process in a wild animal. In N.L. Wallin, B. Merker, & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music, pp. 135–150. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. and Bloom, P. (1990). Natural Language and Natural Selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 707–784.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Richman, B. (2000). How music fixed “nonsense” into significant formulas: On rhythm, repetition, and meaning. In N. L. Wallin, B. Merker and S. Brown (Eds.), The origins of music, pp. 301–314. Cambridge, Mass.: The Mit Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth R. M. and Cheney D. L. (1980). The ontogeny of vervet monkey alarm-calling behavior: A preliminary report. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 54, 37–56.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Snowdon, C. T. and Elowson, A. M. (1992). Ontogeny of primate vocal communication. In Topics in Primatology, Vol. 1, Human Origins. T. Nishida, F.B. M. de Waal, W. McGrew, P. Marler and M. Pickford (Eds.), pp. 279–290. Tokyo University Press. Tokyo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Soma, M., Hasegawa, T. and Okanoya, K. (2005). The evolution of song learning: A review from a biological perspective. Cognitive Studies, 12, 166–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K. (2003). Learning biases and language evolution. In S. Kirby (Ed.) Language Evolution and Computation (pp. 22–31). Proceedings of the Workshop on Language Evolution and Computation, 15th European Summer School on Logic, Language and Information, Vienna.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, J. (1987). The Science of the Singing Voice. Dekalb, Illinois: Northern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, L. W. (1995). Mapping the human brain: past, present, and future. Trends in Neurosciences, 18, 471–474.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Thorpe, W. H. (1961). Bird-Song. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachtmeister, C. -A. (2001). Display in monogamous pairs: A review of empirical data and evolutionary explanations. Animal Behavior, 61, 861–868.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, H. (2004). Birdsong and singing behavior. In H.P. Ziegler and P. Marler (Eds.), Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong, pp. 1–30. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wray, A. (1998). Protolanguage as a holistic system for social interaction. Language and Communication, 18, 47–67.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zeigler, H. P. & Marler, P. (Eds.) (2004). Behavioral Neurobiology of Birdsong. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1993). The fallacy of conventional signaling. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 340, 227–230.

    CrossRef  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. and Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and Permissions

Copyright information

© 2007 Springer-Verlag London Limited

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Merker, B., Okanoya, K. (2007). The Natural History of Human Language: Bridging the Gaps without Magic. In: Lyon, C., Nehaniv, C.L., Cangelosi, A. (eds) Emergence of Communication and Language. Springer, London. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84628-779-4_21

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84628-779-4_21

  • Publisher Name: Springer, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-84628-491-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-84628-779-4

  • eBook Packages: Computer ScienceComputer Science (R0)