Advertisement

How Social Dynamics Shape Our Understanding of Reality

  • Anna AbrahamEmail author
Part of the International and Cultural Psychology book series (ICUP)

Abstract

Abraham examines the intrinsic effortless capacity for human beings to create and immerse themselves in multiple fictional worlds yet still not lose sight of reality. The reality–fiction distinction is discussed both as a developmental phenomenon and as an emergent product of our social experience. It is argued that the reality–fiction distinction is facilitated by spontaneous attributions of personal relevance, which is mainly defined by a variety of cultural factors.

Abraham discusses the experimental neuroimaging evidence for the brain response when making reality–fiction distinctions and demonstrates that the findings are consistent with other culture-related phenomena. The medial prefrontal cortex is highlighted as a key brain region that modulates the determinations of personal relevance. Multiple studies are highlighted to corroborate this postulation.

Abraham concludes with a discussion of the implications of the reality–fiction distinction for intercultural relations. It is argued that an understanding of the reality–fiction distinction could be applied to studies of acculturation, community identity, and prejudice.

Keywords

Reality–fiction Reality–fantasy Narrative comprehension Functional neuroimaging Cognitive neuroscience Default mode Imagination Self-relevance Personal relevance Personal significance  Sociocultural factors 

References

  1. Abraham, A. (2013). The world according to me: Personal relevance and the medial prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 341. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00341.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Abraham, A., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2009). Reality = relevance? Insights from spontaneous modulations of the brain’s default network when telling apart reality from fiction. PloS One, 4(3), e4741. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004741.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Abraham, A., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Meeting George Bush versus meeting Cinderella: The neural response when telling apart what is real from what is fictional in the context of our reality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(6), 965–976. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20059.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Allport, G. W. (1979). The nature of prejudice: 25th anniversary edition (Unabridged ed.). Basic Books.Google Scholar
  5. Badre, D., & Wagner, A. D. (2007). Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 2883–2901. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.015.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bentley, R. A., & O’Brien, M. J. (2012). Cultural evolutionary tipping points in the storage and transmission of information. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 569. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00569.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Segall, M. H., & Dasen, P. R. (2002). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bourhis, R. Y., Moise, L. C., Perreault, S., & Senecal, S. (1997). Towards an interactive acculturation model: A social psychological approach. International Journal of Psychology, 32(6), 369–386. doi: 10.1080/002075997400629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Boyer, P. (1997). Further distinctions between magic, reality, religion, and fiction. Child Development, 68(6), 1012–1014. discussion 1027–1030.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bubic, A., von Cramon, D. Y., & Schubotz, R. I. (2010). Prediction, cognition and the brain. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 25. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00025.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Chiao, J. Y., Iidaka, T., Gordon, H. L., Nogawa, J., Bar, M., Aminoff, E., … Ambady, N. (2008). Cultural specificity in amygdala response to fear faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(12), 2167–2174. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20151.
  12. Doucerain, M., Dere, J., & Ryder, A. G. (2013). Travels in hyper-diversity: Multiculturalism and the contextual assessment of acculturation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 37(6), 686–699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203–235.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Falk, E. B., Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Ascribing beliefs to ingroup and outgroup political candidates: neural correlates of perspective-taking, issue importance and days until the election. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 731–743. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0302.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Fiske, S., & Taylor, S. (2007). Social cognition, from brains to culture (1st ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill Humanities/Social Sciences/Languages.Google Scholar
  16. Heise, D. R. (2014). Cultural variations in sentiments. SpringerPlus, 3, 170. doi: 10.1186/2193-1801-3-170.CrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  17. Hutzler, F. (2013). Reverse inference is not a fallacy per se: Cognitive processes can be inferred from functional imaging data. NeuroImage. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.12.075.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Krienen, F. M., Tu, P.-C., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Clan mentality: Evidence that the medial prefrontal cortex responds to close others. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(41), 13906–13915. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI. 2180-10.2010.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Mar, R. A. (2004). The neuropsychology of narrative: Story comprehension, story production and their interrelation. Neuropsychologia, 42(10), 1414–1434. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.016.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Mitchell, J. P., Macrae, C. N., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Dissociable medial prefrontal contributions to judgments of similar and dissimilar others. Neuron, 50(4), 655–663. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.03.040.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2009). Modulation of cortical midline structures by implicit and explicit self-relevance evaluation. Social Neuroscience, 4(3), 197–211. doi: 10.1080/17470910802250519.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Nell, V. (1988). The psychology of reading for pleasure: Needs and gratifications. Reading Research Quarterly, 23(1), 6–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Northoff, G., Schneider, F., Rotte, M., Matthiae, C., Tempelmann, C., Wiebking, C., … Panksepp, J. (2009). Differential parametric modulation of self-relatedness and emotions in different brain regions. Human Brain Mapping, 30(2), 369–382. doi:10.1002/hbm.20510.
  24. Oatley, K. (2002). Emotions and the story worlds of fiction. In M. Green, J. Strange, & T. Brock (Eds.), Narrative impact: Social and cognitive foundations (pp. 39–70). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  25. Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 922–934. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Poldrack, R. A. (2008). The role of fMRI in cognitive neuroscience: Where do we stand? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 223–227. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Ramnani, N., & Owen, A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 5(3), 184–194. doi: 10.1038/nrn1343.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Rosengren, K. S., & Hickling, A. K. (1994). Seeing is believing: Children’s explanations of commonplace, magical, and extraordinary transformations. Child Development, 65(6), 1605–1626.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Roy, M., Shohamy, D., & Wager, T. D. (2012). Ventromedial prefrontal-subcortical systems and the generation of affective meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 147–156. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.005.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Ryan, M.-L. (1980). Fiction, non-factuals, and the principle of minimal departure. Poetics, 9(4), 403–422. doi: 10.1016/0304-422X(80)90030-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Schmitz, T. W., & Johnson, S. C. (2006). Self-appraisal decisions evoke dissociated dorsal-ventral aMPFC networks. NeuroImage, 30(3), 1050–1058. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.10.030.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Schmitz, T. W., & Johnson, S. C. (2007). Relevance to self: A brief review and framework of neural systems underlying appraisal. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 31(4), 585–596. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.12.003.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074. doi: 10.1068/p2952.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Skolnick, D., & Bloom, P. (2006). What does Batman think about SpongeBob? Children’s understanding of the fantasy/fantasy distinction. Cognition, 101(1), B9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.10.001.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Somerville, L. H., Kelley, W. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (2010). Self-esteem modulates medial prefrontal cortical responses to evaluative social feedback. Cerebral Cortex, 20(12), 3005–3013. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq049.CrossRefPubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Tomaszczyk, J. C., Fernandes, M. A., & MacLeod, C. M. (2008). Personal relevance modulates the positivity bias in recall of emotional pictures in older adults. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15(1), 191–196.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Ülkümen, G., & Thomas, M. (2013). Personal relevance and mental simulation amplify the duration framing effect. Journal of Marketing Research, 50(2), 194–206. doi: 10.1509/jmr.10.0172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Weisberg, D. S., & Goodstein, J. (2009). What belongs in a fictional world? Journal of Cognition and Culture, 9(1–2), 69–78. doi: 10.1163/156853709X414647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Weisberg, D. S., & Sobel, D. M. (2012). Young children discriminate improbable from impossible events in fiction. Cognitive Development, 27(1), 90–98. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.08.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Woolley, J. D. (1997). Thinking about fantasy: Are children fundamentally different thinkers and believers from adults? Child Development, 68(6), 991–1011.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Woolley, J. D., & Van Reet, J. (2006). Effects of context on judgments concerning the reality status of novel entities. Child Development, 77(6), 1778–1793. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00973.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Young, G. (2009). In what sense “familiar”? Examining experiential differences within pathologies of facial recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 628–638. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.06.006.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., & Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1310–1316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Zysset, S., Huber, O., Ferstl, E., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). The anterior frontomedian cortex and evaluative judgment: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 15(4), 983–991. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1008.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, School of Social, Psychological and Communication SciencesLeeds Beckett UniversityLeedsUK

Personalised recommendations