Advertisement

Female Adaptations to Ovulation

  • Lisa L. M. Welling
  • David A. Puts
Chapter
Part of the Evolutionary Psychology book series (EVOLPSYCH)

Abstract

Particularly when compared to many other primates, human females appear to have lost any cues to their ovulatory status at some point during our evolutionary past. Until recently, it was assumed that the timing of peak fertility was hidden from both men and women. However, current evidence suggests that subtle changes in women’s behavior and preferences that correspond with the periovulatory period of a woman’s menstrual cycle persist and may function to promote conception, particularly with higher quality males. Women appear more sexually motivated, are rated as more attractive, and may be better able to attract a mate around ovulation compared to other points in the cycle. Moreover, women report higher attraction to putative indicators of underlying genetic health in men when conception is most likely, which may allow offspring to reap associated genetic benefits. Critically, men seem capable of detecting subtle cues to ovulation, increasing their ratings of the attractiveness of periovulatory women and, according to their romantic partners, engaging in more mate guarding behavior. Altogether, the current research suggests that female ovulation is not entirely concealed.

Keywords

Menstrual Cycle Hormonal Contraceptive Human Female Sexual Motivation Primary Partner 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Adams, D., Burt, A., & Gold, A. R. (1978). Rise in female-initiated sexual activity at ovulation and its suppression by oral contraception. New England Journal of Medicine, 299, 1145–1150.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Alexander, R. D., & Noonan, K. M. (1979). Concealment of ovulation, parental care and human social evolution. In N. Chagnon & W. Irons (Eds.), Evolutionary biology and human social behavior. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
  3. Alvergne, A., & Lummaa, V. (2009). Does the contraceptive pill alter mate choice in humans? Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 25, 171–179.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Andelman, S. J. (1987). Evolution of concealed ovulation in vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). The American Naturalist, 129, 785–799.Google Scholar
  5. Anderson, U. S., Perea, E. F., Vaughn Becker, D., Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., et al. (2010). I only have eyes for you: Ovulation redirects attention (but not memory) to attractive men. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 804–808.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Apicella, C. L., Feinberg, D. R., & Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers. Biology Letters, 3, 682–684.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Bäckström, T., Sanders, D., Leask, R., Davidson, D., Warner, P., & Bancroft, J. (1983). Mood, sexuality, hormones, and the menstrual cycle. II. Hormone levels and their relationship to the premenstrual syndrome. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 503–507.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Baird, D. T., & Fraser, I. S. (1974). Blood production and ovarian secretion rates of estradiol-17β and estrone in women throughout the menstrual cycle. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 38, 1009–1017.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Baker, R. R., & Bellis, M. A. (1993). Human sperm competition: Ejaculate manipulation by females and a function for the female orgasm. Animal Behaviour, 46, 887–909.Google Scholar
  10. Baker, R. R., & Bellis, M. A. (1995). Human sperm competition: Copulation, masturbation and infidelity. London: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
  11. Baum, M. J., Everitt, B. J., Herbert, J., & Keverne, E. B. (1977). Hormonal basis of proceptivity and receptivity in female primates. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 6, 173–192.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Beach, F. A. (1948). Hormones and behavior: A survey of interrelationships between endocrine secretions and patterns of overt response. Oxford, England: Hoeber.Google Scholar
  13. Becker, D., Creutzfeldt, O. D., Schwibbe, M., & Wuttke, W. (1982). Changes in physiological, EEG and psychological parameters in women during the spontaneous menstrual cycle and following oral contraceptives. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 7, 75–90.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Bellis, M. A., & Baker, R. R. (1990). Do females promote sperm competition? Data for humans. Animal Behaviour, 40, 997–999.Google Scholar
  15. Benshoof, L., & Thornhill, R. (1979). The evolution of monogamy and the loss of estrous in humans. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 2, 95–106.Google Scholar
  16. Borries, C., Launhardt, K., Epplen, C., Epplen, J. T., & Winkler, P. (1999). Males as infant protectors in Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) living in multimale groups—Defence pattern, paternity and sexual behaviour. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 46, 350–356.Google Scholar
  17. Brewis, A., & Meyer, M. (2005). Demographic evidence that human ovulation is undetectable (at least in pair bonds). Current Anthropology, 46, 465–471.Google Scholar
  18. Brinsmead-Stockham, K., Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Female sexual orientation and menstrual influences on person perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 729–734.Google Scholar
  19. Bröder, A., & Hohmann, N. (2003). Variations in risk taking behavior over the menstrual cycle: An improved replication. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 397–398.Google Scholar
  20. Broverman, D. M., Vogel, W., Klaiber, E. L., Majcher, D., Shea, D., & Paul, V. (1981). Changes in cognitive task performance across the menstrual cycle. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 95, 646–654.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Bryant, G. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2009). Vocal cues of ovulation in human females. Biology Letters, 5, 12–15.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Burch, R. L., & Gallup, G. G. (2000). Perceptions of paternal resemblance predict family violence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 429–435.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Burley, N. (1979). The evolution of concealed ovulation. American Naturalist, 114, 835–858.Google Scholar
  24. Burriss, R. P., & Little, A. C. (2006). Effects of partner conception risk phase on male perception of dominance in faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 297–305.Google Scholar
  25. Buss, D. M. (1988). From vigilance to violence: Tactics of mate retention in American undergraduates. Ethology and Sociobiology, 9, 291–317.Google Scholar
  26. Buss, D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 559–570.Google Scholar
  27. Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (2008). Attractive women want it all: Good genes, economic investment, parenting proclivities, and emotional commitment. Evolutionary Psychology, 6, 134–146.Google Scholar
  28. Campbell, F. (1960). Birth control and the Christian churches. Population Studies, 14, 131–147.Google Scholar
  29. Campbell, B. G. (1966). Human evolution. Chicago, IL: Aldine.Google Scholar
  30. Cárdenas, R. A., & Harris, L. J. (2007). Do women’s preferences for symmetry change across the menstrual cycle? Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 96–105.Google Scholar
  31. Cerda-Molina, A. L., Hernández-López, L., Chavira, R., Cárdenas, M., Paez-Ponce, D., Cervantes-De la Luz, H., et al. (2006). Endocrine changes in male stumptailed macaques (Macaca arctoides) as a response to odor stimulation with vaginal secretions. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 81–87.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Chavanne, T. J., & Gallup, G. G. (1998). Variation in risk taking behavior among female college students as a function of the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 27–32.Google Scholar
  33. Clarke, P. M. R., Barrett, L., & Henzi, S. P. (2009). What role do olfactory cues play in chacma baboon mating? American Journal of Primatology, 71, 493–502.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Crawford, J. C., Boulet, M., & Drea, C. M. (2011). Smelling wrong: Hormonal contraception in lemurs alters critical female odour cues. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 122–130.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1984). A sociobiological analysis of human infanticide. In G. Hausfater & S. Hrdy (Eds.), Infanticide: Comparative and evolutionary perspectives (pp. 487–502). New York: Aldine Press.Google Scholar
  36. Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1985). Child abuse and other risks of not living with both parents. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6, 197–210.Google Scholar
  37. Daly, M., Wilson, M., & Weghorst, S. J. (1982). Male sexual jealousy. Ethology and Sociobiology, 3, 11–27.Google Scholar
  38. Daniels, D. (1983). The evolution of concealed ovulation and self-deception. Ethology and Sociobiology, 4, 69–87.Google Scholar
  39. DeBruine, L. M. (2004). Resemblance to self increases the appeal of child faces to both men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 142–154.Google Scholar
  40. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Crawford, J. R., Welling, L. L. M., & Little, A. C. (2010). The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: Cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for masculinized male faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277, 2405–2410.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Crawford, J. R., & Welling, L. L. M. (2011). Further evidence for regional variation in women’s masculinity preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 813–814.PubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  42. Doty, R. L., Ford, M. E., Preti, G., & Huggins, G. R. (1975). Changes in the intensity and pleasantness of human vaginal odors during the menstrual cycle. Science, 190, 1316–1318.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Durante, K. M., & Li, N. P. (2009). Oestradiol level and opportunistic mating in women. Biology Letters, 5, 179–182.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Durante, K. M., Li, N. P., & Haselton, M. G. (2008). Changes in women’s choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: Naturalistic and laboratory task-based evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1451–1460.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Law Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., et al. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 215–222.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Fessler, D. M. T. (2003a). No time to eat: An adaptationist account of periovulatory behavioral changes. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 78, 3–21.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  47. Fessler, D. M. T. (2003b). Rape is not less frequent during the ovulatory phase of the menstrual cycle. Sexualities, Evolution and Gender, 5, 127–147.Google Scholar
  48. Fessler, D. M. T., & Navarrete, C. D. (2003). Domain-specific variation in disgust sensitivity across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 406–417.Google Scholar
  49. Fetchenhauer, D., & Rohde, P. A. (2002). Evolutionary personality psychology and victimology: Sex differences in risk attitudes and short-term orientation and their relation to sex differences in victimizations. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 233–244.Google Scholar
  50. Fink, B., & Penton-Voak, I. (2002). Evolutionary psychology of facial attractiveness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 154–158.Google Scholar
  51. Fisher, M. L. (2004). Female intrasexual competition decreases female facial attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 271, S283–S285.Google Scholar
  52. Fisher, M. L., & Cox, A. (2009). The influence of female attractiveness on competitor derogation. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 141–155.Google Scholar
  53. Frost, P. (1994). Preference for darker faces in photographs at different phases of the menstrual cycle: Preliminary assessment of evidence for a hormonal relationship. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 507–514.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Gangestad, S. W., Garver-Apgar, C. E., Simpson, J. A., & Cousins, A. J. (2007). Changes in women’s mate preferences across the cycle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 151–163.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–644.Google Scholar
  56. Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Christensen, P. N. (2004). Women’s preferences for male behavioral displays change across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, 15, 203–207.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265, 927–933.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2008). Human oestrus. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275, 991–1000.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver, C. E. (2002). Changes in women’s sexual interests and their partners’ mate-retention tactics across the menstrual cycle: Evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269, 975–982.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005). Women’s sexual interests across the ovulatory cycle depend on primary partner developmental instability. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272, 2023–2027.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2010). Men’s facial masculinity predicts changes in their female partners’ sexual interests across the cycle, whereas men’s intelligence does not. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31, 412–424.Google Scholar
  62. Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2008). Hormonal correlates of women’s mid-cycle preference for the scent of symmetry. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 223–232.Google Scholar
  63. Garver-Apgar, C. E., Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., Miller, R. D., & Olp, J. (2006). Major histocompatibility complex alleles, sexual responsivity, and unfaithfulness in romantic couples. Psychological Science, 17, 830–835.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Gildersleeve, K. A., Haselton, M. G., Larson, C. M., & Pillsworth, E. G. (2012). Body odor attractiveness as a cue of impending ovulation in women: Evidence from a study using hormone-confirmed ovulation. Hormones and Behavior, 61, 157–166.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Giraldi, A., Marson, L., Nappi, R., Pfaus, J., Traish, A. M., Vardi, Y., et al. (2004). Physiology of female sexual function: Animal models. The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 1, 237–253.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Gong, E. J., Garrel, D., & Calloway, D. H. (1989). Menstrual cycle and voluntary food intake. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 49, 252–258.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Grammer, K. (1993). 5-α-androst-16en-3α-on: A male pheromone? A brief report. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 201–208.Google Scholar
  68. Grammer, K., Jutte, A., & Fischmann, B. (1997). Der kampf der geschlechter und der krieg der signale. In B. Karnitschneider (Ed.), Liebe, Lust, und Leidenschaft. Sexualitat im Spiegal der Wissenschaft. Stuttgart: Hirzel.Google Scholar
  69. Guéguen, N. (2009a). Menstrual cycle phases and female receptivity to a courtship solicitation: An evaluation in a nightclub. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 351–355.Google Scholar
  70. Guéguen, N. (2009b). The receptivity of women to courtship solicitation across the menstrual cycle: A field experiment. Biological Psychology, 80, 321–324.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  71. Harvey, S. M. (1987). Female sexual behavior: Fluctuations during the menstrual cycle. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 31, 101–110.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Haselton, M. G., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Conditional expression of women’s desires and men’s mate guarding across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 509–518.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Haselton, M. G., & Gildersleeve, K. A. (2011). Can men detect ovulation? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 87–92.Google Scholar
  74. Haselton, M. G., Mortezaie, M., Pillsworth, E. G., Bleske-Rechek, A. L., & Frederick, D. A. (2007). Ovulatory shifts in human female ornamentation: Near ovulation, women dress to impress. Hormones and Behavior, 51, 40–45.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  75. Havlíček, J., Dvořáková, R., Bartoš, L., & Flegr, J. (2006). Non-advertized does not mean concealed: Body odour changes across the human menstrual cycle. Ethology, 112, 81–90.Google Scholar
  76. Havlíček, J., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 497–512.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Havlíček, J., Roberts, S. C., & Flegr, J. (2005). Women’s preference for dominant male odour: Effects of menstrual cycle and relationship status. Biology Letters, 1, 256–259.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  78. Heistermann, M., Ziegler, T., van Schaik, C. P., Launhardt, K., Winkler, P., & Hodges, J. K. (2001). Loss of oestrus, concealed ovulation and paternity confusion in free-ranging Hanuman langurs. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 268, 2445–2451.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Hellhammer, D. H., Hubert, W., & Schürmeyer, T. (1985). Changes in saliva testosterone after psychological stimulation in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 10, 77–81.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Henderson, J. J. A., & Anglin, J. M. (2003). Facial attractiveness predicts longevity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 351–356.Google Scholar
  81. Hill, S. E., & Durante, K. M. (2009). Do women feel worse to look their best? Testing the relationship between self-esteem and fertility status across the menstrual cycle. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1592–1601.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Hrdy, S. B. (1979). Infanticide among mammals: A review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethology and Sociobiology, 1, 13–40.Google Scholar
  83. Hrdy, S. B., & Whitten, P. L. (1987). Patterning of sexual activity. In B. S. Smuts, D. L. Cheney, R. M. Seyfarth, R. W. Wrangham, & T. T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 370–384). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  84. Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: Evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 251–267.Google Scholar
  85. Johnston, L., Miles, L., & Macrae, C. N. (2008). Was that a man? Sex identification as a function of menstrual cycle and masculinity. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 1185–1194.Google Scholar
  86. Jokela, M. (2009). Physical attractiveness and reproductive success in humans: Evidence from the late 20th century United States. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 342–350.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  87. Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., & Law Smith, M. J. (2008). Effects of menstrual cycle phase on face preferences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 78–84.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L., DeBruine, L. M., Feinberg, D. R., Law Smith, M. J., et al. (2005). Commitment to relationships and preferences for femininity and apparent health in faces are strongest on days of the menstrual cycle when progesterone level is high. Hormones and Behavior, 48, 283–290.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Jones, B. C., Perrett, D. I., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L., Cornwell, R. E., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2005). Menstrual cycle, pregnancy and oral contraceptive use alter attraction to apparent health in faces. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272, 347–354.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  90. Jones, B. C., Vukovic, J., Little, A. C., Roberts, S. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2011). Circum-menopausal changes in women’s preferences for sexually dimorphic shape cues in peer-aged faces. Biological Psychology, 87, 453–455.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Kendrick, K. M., & Dixson, A. F. (1985). Effects of oestradiol 17β, progesterone and testosterone upon proceptivity and receptivity in ovariectomized common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Physiology and Behavior, 34, 123–128.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Kirchengast, S., & Gartner, M. (2002). Changes in fat distribution (WHR) and body weight across the menstrual cycle. Collegium Antropologicum, 26, 47–57.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Koehler, N., Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Zebrowitz, L. A. (2006). Do cyclic changes in women’s face preferences target cues to long-term health? Social Cognition, 24, 641–656.Google Scholar
  94. Krug, R., Finn, M., Pietrowsky, R., Fehm, H. L., & Born, J. (1996). Jealousy, general creativity, and coping with social frustration during the menstrual cycle. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 181–199.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  95. Krug, R., Stamm, U., Pietrowsky, R., Fehm, H. L., & Born, J. (1994). Effects of menstrual cycle on creativity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 19, 21–31.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Kuukasjärvi, S., Eriksson, C. J. P., Koskela, E., Mappes, T., Nissinen, K., & Rantala, M. J. (2004). Attractiveness of women’s body odors over the menstrual cycle: The role of oral contraceptives and receiver sex. Behavioral Ecology, 15, 579–584.Google Scholar
  97. Laeng, B., & Falkenberg, L. (2007). Women’s pupillary responses to sexually significant others during the hormonal cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 52, 520–530.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., & Musselman, L. (1994). What is average and what is not average about attractive faces. Psychological Science, 5, 214–219.Google Scholar
  99. Latz, L. J. (1939). The rhythm of sterility and fertility in women. Chicago, IL: Latz Foundation.Google Scholar
  100. Lee, R. B., & DeVore, I. (1968). Man the hunter. Oxford, England: Aldine.Google Scholar
  101. Lie, H. C., Rhodes, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2008). Genetic diversity revealed in human faces. Evolution, 62, 2473–2486.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. Little, A. C., Burriss, R. P., Tufte, A. D., & Jones, B. C. (2006). Hormone mediated preferences for bodies and faces. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 24, 270.Google Scholar
  103. Little, A. C., Cohen, D. L., Jones, B. C., & Belsky, J. (2007). Human preferences for facial masculinity change with relationship type and environmental harshness. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 61, 967–973.Google Scholar
  104. Little, A. C., & Jones, B. C. (2011). Variation in facial masculinity and symmetry preferences across the menstrual cycle is moderated by relationship context. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37, 999–1008.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & Burriss, R. P. (2007). Preferences for masculinity in male bodies change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 51, 633–639.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  106. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2007). Preferences for symmetry in faces change across the menstrual cycle. Biological Psychology, 76, 209–216.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & DeBruine, L. M. (2008). Preferences for variation in masculinity in real male faces change across the menstrual cycle: Women prefer more masculine faces when they are more fertile. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 478–482.Google Scholar
  108. Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., Penton-Voak, I. S., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2002). Partnership status and the temporal context of relationships influence human female preferences for sexual dimorphism in male face shape. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269, 1095–1100.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. López, H. H., Hay, A. C., & Conklin, P. H. (2009). Attractive men induce testosterone and cortisol release in women. Hormones and Behavior, 56, 84–92.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Lukaszewski, A. W., & Roney, J. R. (2009). Estimated hormones predict women’s mate preferences for dominant personality traits. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 191–196.Google Scholar
  111. Macrae, C. N., Alnwick, K. A., Milne, A. B., & Schloerscheidt, A. M. (2002). Person perception across the menstrual cycle: Hormonal influences of social-cognitive functioning. Psychological Science, 13, 532–536.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Manning, J. T., Scutt, D., Whitehouse, G. H., & Leinster, S. J. (1997). Breast asymmetry and phenotypic quality in women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 223–236.Google Scholar
  113. Manning, J. T., Scutt, D., Whitehouse, G. H., Leinster, S. J., & Walton, J. M. (1996). Asymmetry and the menstrual cycle in women. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17, 129–143.Google Scholar
  114. Manson, W. C. (1986). Sexual cyclicity and concealed ovulation. Journal of Human Evolution, 15, 21–30.Google Scholar
  115. Marlowe, F. W. (2004). Is human ovulation concealed? Evidence from conception beliefs in a hunter-gatherer society. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 33, 427–432.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Matteo, S., & Rissman, E. F. (1984). Increased sexual activity during the midcycle portion of the human menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 18, 249–255.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 353–397.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2010). Scent of a woman: Men’s testosterone responses to olfactory ovulation cues. Psychological Science, 21, 276–283.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  119. Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2011). Ovulation as a male mating prime: Subtle signs of women’s fertility influence men’s mating cognition and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 295–308.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Miller, G. F., & Todd, P. M. (1998). Mate choice turns cognitive. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 190–198.Google Scholar
  121. Miller, G., Tybur, J. M., & Jordan, B. D. (2007). Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap dancers: Economic evidence for human estrus? Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 375–381.Google Scholar
  122. Møller, A. P., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Bilateral symmetry and sexual selection: A meta-analysis. American Naturalist, 151, 174–192.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  123. Morris, N. M., & Udry, J. R. (1970). Variations in pedometer activity during the menstrual cycle. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 35, 199–201.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Morris, N. M., & Udry, J. R. (1982). Epidemiological patterns of sexual behavior in the menstrual cycle. In R. C. Friedman (Ed.), Behavior and the menstrual cycle (pp. 129–154). New York: Marcel Dekker.Google Scholar
  125. Nunn, C. L. (1999). The evolution of exaggerated sexual swellings in primates and the graded-signal hypothesis. Animal Behavior, 58, 229–246.Google Scholar
  126. O’Toole, A. J., Deffenbacher, K. A., Valentin, D., McKee, K., Huff, D., & Abdi, H. (1998). The perception of face gender: The role of stimulus structure in recognition and classification. Memory and Cognition, 26, 146–160.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  127. Pawłowski, B. (1999). Loss of oestrus and concealed ovulation in human evolution: The case against the sexual selection hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 40, 257–276.Google Scholar
  128. Pawlowski, B., & Jasienska, G. (2005). Women’s preferences for sexual dimorphism in height depend on menstrual cycle phase and expected duration of relationship. Biological Psychology, 70, 38–43.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2000). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically—Further evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21, 39–48.Google Scholar
  130. Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: Perceived personality and shifting female preferences for male traits across the menstrual cycle. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 30, 219–259.Google Scholar
  131. Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., Murray, L. K., et al. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face preference. Nature, 399, 741–742.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Perrett, D. I., Lee, K. J., Penton-Voak, I. S., Rowland, D. R., Yoshikawa, S., Burt, D. M., et al. (1998). Effects of sexual dimorphism on facial attractiveness. Nature, 394, 884–887.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  133. Peters, M., Rhodes, G., & Simmons, L. W. (2008). Does attractiveness in men provide clues to semen quality? Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21, 572–579.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  134. Pillsworth, E. G., & Haselton, M. G. (2006). Male sexual attractiveness predicts differential ovulatory shifts in female extra-pair attraction and male mate retention. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 247–258.Google Scholar
  135. Pipitone, R. N., & Gallup, G. G. (2008). Women’s voice attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 268–274.Google Scholar
  136. Platek, S. M., Burch, R. L., Panyavin, I. S., Wasserman, B. H., & Gallup, G. G. (2002). Reactions to children’s faces—Resemblance affects males more than females. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23, 159–166.Google Scholar
  137. Platek, S. M., Critton, S. R., Burch, R. L., Frederick, D. A., Myers, T. E., & Gallup, G. G. (2003). How much paternal resemblance is enough? Sex differences in hypothetical investment decisions but not in the detection of resemblance. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 81–87.Google Scholar
  138. Provost, M. P., Quinsey, V. L., & Troje, N. F. (2008). Differences in gait across the menstrual cycle and their attractiveness to men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 598–604.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  139. Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 388–397.Google Scholar
  140. Puts, D. A. (2006). Cyclic variation in women’s preferences for masculine traits: Potential hormonal causes. Human Nature, 17, 114–127.Google Scholar
  141. Puts, D. A., Bailey, D. H., Cárdenas, R. A., Burriss, R. P., Welling, L. L. M., Wheatley, J. R., et al. (2013). Women’s attractiveness changes with estradiol and progesterone across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 63(1), 13–19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.007
  142. Regan, P. C. (1996). Rhythms of desire: The association between menstrual cycle phases and female sexual desire. The Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality, 5, 145–156.Google Scholar
  143. Rhodes, G., Chan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., & Simmons, L. W. (2003). Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 270, S93–S95.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  144. Rhodes, G., Simmons, L. W., & Peters, M. (2005). Attractiveness and sexual behavior: Does attractiveness enhance mating success? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 186–201.Google Scholar
  145. Rikowski, A., & Grammer, K. (1999). Human body odour, symmetry and attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 266, 869–874.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  146. Robbins, M. M. (1995). A demographic analysis of male life history and social structure of mountain gorillas. Behaviour, 132, 21–47.Google Scholar
  147. Roberts, S. C., Gosling, L. M., Carter, V., & Petrie, M. (2008). MHC-correlated odour preferences in humans and the use of oral contraceptives. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275, 2715–2722.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  148. Roberts, S. C., Havlicek, J., Flegr, J., Hruskova, M., Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., et al. (2004). Female facial attractiveness increases during the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 271, S270–S272.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  149. Roberts, S. C., Little, A. C., Gosling, L. M., Perrett, D. I., Carter, V., Jones, B. C., et al. (2005). MHC-heterozygosity and human facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 213–226.Google Scholar
  150. Röder, S., Brewer, G., & Fink, B. (2009). Menstrual cycle shifts in women’s self-perception and motivation: A daily report method. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 616–619.Google Scholar
  151. Ronay, R., & von Hippel, W. (2010). The presence of an attractive woman elevates testosterone and physical risk taking in young men. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, 57–64.Google Scholar
  152. Roney, J. R., Lukaszewski, A. W., & Simmons, Z. L. (2007). Rapid endocrine responses of young men to social interactions with young women. Hormones and Behavior, 52, 326–333.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Roney, J. R., Mahler, S. V., & Maestripieri, D. (2003). Behavioral and hormonal responses of men to brief interactions with women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 365–375.Google Scholar
  154. Roney, J. R., & Simmons, Z. L. (2008). Women’s estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men’s testosterone. Hormones and Behavior, 53, 14–19.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  155. Roney, J. R., & Simmons, Z. L. (2012). Men smelling women: Null effects of exposure to ovulatory sweat on men’s testosterone. Evolutionary Psychology, 10, 703–713.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  156. Rosen, M. L., & López, H. H. (2009). Menstrual cycle shifts in attentional bias for courtship language. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 131–140.Google Scholar
  157. Rubin, H. B., Henson, D. E., Falvo, R. E., & High, R. W. (1979). The relationship between men’s endogenous levels of testosterone and their penile responses to erotic stimuli. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 17, 305–312.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  158. Rupp, H. A., James, T. W., Ketterson, E. D., Sengelaub, D. R., Janssen, E., & Heiman, J. R. (2009). Neural activation in women in response to masculinized male faces: Mediation by hormones and psychosexual factors. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 1–10.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  159. Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (2001). Human mate poaching: Tactics and temptations for infiltrating existing mateships. Interpersonal Relations and Group Processes, 80, 894–917.Google Scholar
  160. Schwarz, S., & Hassebrauck, M. (2008). Self-perceived and observed variations in women’s attractiveness throughout the menstrual cycle—A diary study. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29, 282–288.Google Scholar
  161. Scutt, D., & Manning, J. T. (1996). Symmetry and ovulation in women. Human Reproduction, 11, 2477–2480.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Shackelford, T. K., Besser, A., & Goetz, A. T. (2008). Personality, marital satisfaction, and probability of marital infidelity. Individual Differences Research, 6, 13–25.Google Scholar
  163. Shackelford, T. K., Goetz, A. T., Buss, D. M., Euler, H. A., & Hoier, S. (2005). When we hurt the ones we love: Predicting violence against women from men’s mate retention. Personal Relationships, 12, 447–463.Google Scholar
  164. Sheldon, M. S., Cooper, M. L., Geary, D. C., Hoard, M., & DeSoto, M. C. (2006). Fertility cycle patterns in motives for sexual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1659–1673.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  165. Sillen-Tullberg, B., & Møller, A. P. (1993). The relationship between concealed ovulation and mating systems in anthropoid primates: A phylogenetic analysis. American Naturalist, 141, 1–25.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  166. Singh, D., & Bronstad, P. M. (2001). Female body odour is a potential cue to ovulation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 268, 797–801.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  167. Slob, A. K., Bax, C. M., Hop, W. C. J., Rowland, D. L., & van der Werff ten Bosch, J. J. (1996). Sexual arousability and the menstrual cycle. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 21, 545–558.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  168. Smith, T. E., & Abbott, D. H. (1998). Behavioral discrimination between circumgenital odor from peri-ovulatory dominant and anovulatory female common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 46, 265–284.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  169. Soler, C., Núñez, M., Gutiérrez, R., Núñez, J., Medina, P., Sancho, M., et al. (2003). Facial attractiveness in men provides clues to semen quality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 199–207.Google Scholar
  170. Soltis, J., Thomsen, R., Matsubayashi, K., & Takenaka, O. (2000). Infanticide by resident males and female counter-strategies in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 48, 195–202.Google Scholar
  171. Stanislaw, H., & Rice, F. J. (1988). Correlation between sexual desire and menstrual cycle characteristics. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 17, 499–508.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  172. Strassmann, B. I. (1981). Sexual selection, paternal care, and concealed ovulation in humans. Ethology and Sociobiology, 2, 31–40.Google Scholar
  173. Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  174. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1993). Human facial beauty: Averageness, symmetry, and parasite resistance. Human Nature, 4, 237–269.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  175. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (1999). The scent of symmetry: A human sex pheromone that signals fitness? Evolution and Human Behavior, 20, 175–201.Google Scholar
  176. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2003). Do women have evolved adaptation for extra-pair copulation? In E. Voland & K. Grammer (Eds.), Evolutionary aesthetics (pp. 341–368). Heidelberg: Springer.Google Scholar
  177. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2006). Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 131–144.Google Scholar
  178. Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). The evolutionary biology of human female sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  179. Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Miller, R., Scheyd, G., McCollough, J. K., & Franklin, M. (2003). Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women (Homo sapiens). Behavioral Ecology, 14, 668–678.Google Scholar
  180. Thornhill, R., & Møller, A. P. (1997). Developmental stability, disease and medicine. Biological Reviews, 72, 497–548.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  181. Turke, P. W. (1984). Effects of ovulatory concealment and synchrony on protohominid mating systems and parental roles. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 33–44.Google Scholar
  182. van Schaik, C. P., Hodges, J. K., & Nunn, C. L. (2000). Paternity confusion and the ovarian cycles of female primates. In C. P. van Schaik & C. H. Janson (Eds.), Infanticide by males and its implications (pp. 361–387). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  183. van Schaik, C. P., van Noordwijk, M. A., & Nunn, C. L. (1999). Sex and social evolution in primates. In P. C. Lee (Ed.), Primate socioecology (pp. 204–240). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  184. Volk, A., & Quinsey, V. L. (2002). The influence of infant facial cues on adoption preferences. Human Nature, 13, 437–455.Google Scholar
  185. Vukovic, J., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Feinberg, D. R., & Welling, L. L. M. (2009). Circum-menopausal effects on women’s judgements of facial attractiveness. Biology Letters, 5, 62–64.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  186. Wallen, K., & Zehr, J. L. (2004). Hormones and history: The evolution and development of primate female sexuality. Journal of Sex Research, 41, 101–112.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  187. Waynforth, D. (1998). Fluctuating asymmetry and human male life-history traits in rural Belize. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265, 1497–1501.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  188. Wedekind, C., & Füri, S. (1997). Body odour preferences in men and women: Do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 264, 1471–1479.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  189. Welling, L. L. M. (2013). Psychobehavioral effects of hormonal contraceptive use. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(3), 718–742.Google Scholar
  190. Welling, L. L. M., Burriss, R. P., & Puts, D. A. (2011). Mate retention behavior modulates men’s preferences for self-resemblance in infant faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 118–126.Google Scholar
  191. Welling, L. L. M., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Conway, C. A., Law Smith, M. J., Little, A. C., et al. (2007). Raised salivary testosterone in women is associated with increased attraction to masculine faces. Hormones and Behavior, 52, 156–161.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  192. Wilcox, A. J., Baird, D. D., Dunson, D. B., McConnaughey, D. R., Kesner, J. S., & Weinberg, C. R. (2004). On the frequency of intercourse around ovulation: Evidence for biological influences. Human Reproduction, 19, 1539–1543.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  193. Wildt, L., Kissler, S., Licht, P., & Becker, W. (1998). Sperm transport in the human female genital tract and its modulation by oxytocin as assessed by hysterosalpingoscintigraphy, hysterotonography, electrohysterography and Doppler sonography. Human Reproduction Update, 4, 655–666.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  194. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1987). Risk of maltreatment of children living with stepparents. In R. J. Gelles & J. B. Lancaster (Eds.), Child abuse and neglect: Biosocial dimensions (pp. 215–232). Hawthorne, New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  195. Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (2002). Infanticide. In M. Pagel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  196. Wrangham, R. (1993). The evolution of sexuality in chimpanzees and bonobos. Human Nature, 4, 47–79.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  197. Zervomanolakis, I., Ott, H. W., Müller, J., Seeber, B. E., Friess, S. C., Mattle, V., et al. (2009). Uterine mechanisms of ipsilateral directed spermatozoa transport: Evidence for a contribution of the utero-ovarian countercurrent system. European Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology and Reproductive Biology, 144, S45–S49.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  198. Zillmann, D., Schweitzer, K. J., & Mundorf, N. (1994). Menstrual cycle variation of women’s interest in erotica. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 579–597.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  199. Zitzmann, M., & Nieschlag, E. (2001). Testosterone levels in healthy men and the relation to behavioural and physical characteristics: Facts and constructs. European Journal of Endocrinology, 144, 183–197.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyOakland UniversityRochesterUSA
  2. 2.Department of AnthropologyPennsylvania State UniversityUniversity ParkUSA

Personalised recommendations