Skip to main content

Freud, Jung, and the Evolution and Duality of the Mind and Brain

  • Chapter
The Right Brain and the Unconscious

Abstract

For many thousands of years human beings have speculated on the possible existence of a nonconscious psychic or spiritual domain from which intuitive leaps, inspirations, dreams, and forbidden impulses originate. Some of the ancients believed that the ultimate source of these mysterious forms of experience were the various gods who populated the heavens. For their own unknown reasons and purposes, the gods transmitted these thoughts, dreams, and ideas into human hearts and heads. There was no unconscious mind. There were unconscious gods. For a primitive human being to truly know himself and his destiny, he had to supplicate these gods and study the dreams they sent.

(From below) cried ID: “If thou be Christ, save thyself and us.” But Superego rebuked ID, saying: “Dost not thou fear God?”

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. H. Goodglass and E. Kaplan, Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination ( Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger, 1982 ).

    Google Scholar 

  2. D. N. Levine and E. Sweet, “Localization of Lesions in Broca’s Motor Aphasia.” In Localization in Neuropsychology, A. Kertesz, Ed. (pp. 185–207 ) ( New York: Academic Press, 1983 ).

    Google Scholar 

  3. R. Joseph, Neuropsychology, Neuropsychiatry, and Behavioral Neurology (New York: Plenum Press, 1990); R. Joseph, “The Right Cerebral Hemisphere: Emotion, Music, Visual-Spatial Skills, Body Image, Dreams and Awareness,” Journal of Clinical Psychology 44 (1988a), 630–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. A. Yamadori, U. Osumi, S. Mashuara, and M. Okuto, “Preservation of Singing in Broca’s Aphasia,” Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 40 (1977), 221–224.

    Google Scholar 

  5. One famous example was the composer and pianist Maurice Ravel, who suffered an injury to the left half of his brain in an auto accident. This injury resulted in apraxia, agraphia, and Wernicke’s aphasia. Nevertheless, he had no difficulty recognizing various musical compositions, was able to detect even minor errors when compositions were played, and was able to correct those errors by playing them correctly on the piano.

    Google Scholar 

  6. Joseph, 1988a, 1990; R. Joseph, “The Neuropsychology of Development: Hemispheric Laterality, Limbic Language and the Origin of Thought,” Journal of Clinical Psychology 33 (1988), 3–33.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Joseph, 1982, 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  8. H. M. Ehrlichman, J. S. Antrobus, and M. Wiener, “EEG Asymmetry and Sleep Mentation during REM and NREM,” Brain and Cognition 4 (1985), 477–485.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. D. Hodoba, “Paradoxic Sleep Facilitation by Interictal Epileptic Activity of Right Temporal Origin,” Biological Psychiatry 21 (1986), 1267–1278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. N. H. Kerr and D. Foulkes, “Right Hemisphere Mediation of Dream Visualization: A Case Study,” Cortex 17 (1981), 603–611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. L. Goldstein, N. W. Stolzfus, and J. F. Gardocki, “Changes in Inter-hemispheric Amplitude Relationships in the EEG during Sleep,” Physiology and Behavior 8 (1972), 811–815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. R. Broughton, “Human Consciousness and Sleep/Waking Rhythms: A Review and Some Neuropsychological Considerations,” Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology 4 (1982), 193–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. J. P. Banquet, “Inter-and Intrahemispheric Relationships of the EEG Activity during Sleep in Man,” Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 55 (1983), 51–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Joseph, 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  15. S. Blumstein and W. E. Cooper, “Hemispheric Processing of Intonational Contours,” Cortex 10 (1974), 146–158; F. Boiler, M. Cole, P. B. Vrtunski, M. Patterson, and V. Kim, “Paralinguistic Aspects of Auditory Comprehension in Aphasia,” Brain and Language 9 (1979), 164–174; D. Breitling, W. Guenther, and P Rondot, ‘Auditory Perception of Music Measured by Brain Electrical Activity Mapping,“ Neuropsychologia 25 (1987), 765–774; M. P. Bryden, R. G. Ley, and J. H. Sugarman, A Left-Ear Advantage for Identifying the Emotional Quality of Tonal Sequences,” Neuropsychologia 20 (1982), 83–87; A. Carmon and I. Nachshon, “Ear Asymmetry in Perception of Emotional Non-Verbal Stimuli,” Acta Psychologica 37 (1973) 351–357; A. Gates and J. L. Bradshaw, “The Role of the Cerebral Hemispheres in Music,” Brain and Language 3 (1977), 451–460; H. W. Gardan, “Hemispheric Asymmetries in the Perception of Musical Chords,” Cortex 6 (1970), 387–398; H. R. McFar-land and D. Fortin, ‘Amusia due to Right Temporal-Parietal Infarct,“ Archives of Neurology 39 (1982), 725–727; K. M. Heilman, D. Bowers, L. Speedie, and H. B. Coslett, ”Comprehension of Affective and Noneffective Prosody,“ Neurology 34 (1984), 917–921; K. Heilman, R. Scholes, and R. T. Watson, ‘Auditory Affective Agnosia,” Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 38 (1975), 69–72; F. L. King and D. Kimura, “Left Ear Superiority in Dichotic Perception of Vocal Nonverbal Sounds/7 Canadian Journal of Psychology 26 (1972), 111–116; C. Knox, and D. Kimura, ”Cerebral Processing of Nonverbal Sounds in Boys and Girls,“ Neuropsychologia 8 (1970), 227–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. About the relations between music, math, and geometric space, Pythagoras, the great Greek mathematician, argued almost two thousand years ago that music is numerical, the expression of number in sound. In fact, long before the advent of digital recordings, the Hindus and Babylonians and then Pythagoras and his followers translated music into numbers and geometric proportions. For example, by dividing a vibrating string into various ratios, they discovered that several very pleasing musical intervals could be produced. Hence, the ratio 1:2 yielded an octave, 2:3 a fifth, 3:4 a fourth, 4:5 a major third, and 5:6 a minor third. The harmonic system used in the nineteenth century by various composers was based on these same ratios. Indeed, Bartok used these ratios in his musical compositions. These same musical ratios, the Pythagoreans discovered, were also able to reproduce themselves. That is, the ratio can reproduce itself within itself and form a unique geometrical configuration, which Pythagoras and the ancient Greeks referred to as the golden ratio or golden rectangle. It was postulated to have divine inspirational origins. Indeed, music itself was thought by early humans to be magical, and musicians were believed by the ancient Greeks to be “prophets favored by the Gods.” This same “golden rectangle” is found in nature, for example, in the chambered nautilus shell, in the shell of a snail, and in the cochlea of the ear. The geometric proportions of the golden rectangle were also used in designing the Parthenon in Athens, and by Ptolemy in developing the “tonal calendar” and the “tonal Zodiac,” the scale of ratios being “bent round in a circle.” In fact, the first cosmologies, such as those developed by the ancient Egyptians, Hindus, Babylonians, and Greeks, were based on musical ratios. Pythagoras and, later, Plato applied these same “musical proportions” to their theory of numbers, planetary motion, and the science of stereometry, the gauging of solids. Indeed, Pythagoras attempted to deduce the size, speed, distance, and orbit of the planets based on musical ratios as well as estimates of the sounds (e.g., the pitch and harmony) generated by their movement through space, that is, “the music of the spheres.” Interestingly, the famous mathematician and physicist Johannes Kepler, in describing his laws of planetary motion, also referred to them as being based on the “music of the spheres.” Thus, music seems to have certain geometric properties, such as are expressed via ratios, and Pythagoras, the “father” of arithmetic, geometry, and trigonometry, believed music to be geometric. As we know, geometry is used in the measurement of land and the demarcation of boundaries, and, thus, in the analysis of space, shape, points, lines, angles, surfaces, and configuration—capacities mediated by the right brain. In nature one form of musical expression—that is, the song of most birds—is also produced for geometric purposes: A bird sings not “for joy” but to signal others of impending threat, to attract mates, to indicate direction and location, to stake out territory, and to warn away others that attempt to intrude on the bird’s space. If we assume that long before humans sang their first song, the first songs and musical compositions were created by our fine feathered friends (sounds that inspired mimicry by humans), it appears that musical production was first and foremost emotional and motivational, and directly related to the geometry of space, that is, the demarcation of one’s territory. Emotion and geometry are characteristics that music still retains, and all are linked to the right half of the brain.

    Google Scholar 

  17. D. Benson and M. Barton, “Disturbances in Constructional Ability,” Cortex 6 (1970), 19–46; A. Benton, “Visuoperceptive, visuospatial and visuocon-structive disorders.” Clinical Neuropsychology, K. M. Heilman and E. Valen-stein, Eds. (pp. 186–232) (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1979); F. W. Black and B. A. Bernard, “Constructional Apraxia as a Function of Lesion Locus and Size in Patients with Focal Brain Damage,” Cortex 20 (1984), 111— 120; R. Calvanio, E N. Petrone, and D. N. Levine, “Left Visual Spatial Neglect Is Both Environment-Centered and Body-Centered,” Neurology 37 (1987), 1179–1183; E. DeRenzi, Disorder of Space Exploration and Cognition (New York: Wiley, 1982); H. Hecaen and M. L. Albert, Human Neuropsychology (New York: Wiley, 1978); Y. Kim, L. Morrow, D. Passafiume, and F. Boiler, “Visuoperceptual and Visuomotor Abilities and Locus of Lesion,” Neuropsychologia 2 (1984), 177–185.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Joseph, 1988, 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  19. H. Gardner, H. H. Brownell, W. Wapner, and D. Michelow, “Missing the Point: The Role of the Right Hemisphere in the Processing of Complex Linguistic Materials.” In Cognitive Processing in the Right Hemisphere E. Perceman, Ed. (New York: Academic Press, 1983); H. H. Brownell, H. H. Potter, and A. M. Bihrle, “Inference Deficits in Right Brain-Damaged Patients,” Brain and Language 27 (1986), 310–321; M. Cicone, W. Wapner, and H. Gardner, “Sensitivity to Emotional Expressions and Situations in Organic Patients,” Cortex 16 (1980), 145–158.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Joseph, 1988, 1990; E. DeRenzi, “Prosopagnosia in Two Patients with CT- Scan Evidence of Damage Confined to the Right Hemisphere,” Neuropsychologia 24 (1986), 385–389; T. Landis, J. L. Cummings, L. Christen, J. E. Bogen, and H.-G. Imhof, ‘Are Unilateral Right Posterior Cerebral Lesions Sufficient to Cause Prosopagnosia? Clinical and Radiological Findings in Six Additional Patients,“ Cortex 22 (1986), 243–252; D. N. Levine, ”Prosopagnosia and Visual Object Agnosia: A Behavioral Study,“ Brain and Language 5 (1978), 341–365; R. G. Ley and M. P. Bryden, ”Hemispheric Differences in Processing Emotions and Faces,“ Brain and Language 7 (1979), 127–138.

    Google Scholar 

  21. J. L. Bradshaw, N. C. Nettleton, and K. Spher, “Braille Reading and Left and Right Hemispace,” Neuropsychologia 20 (1982), 493–500; A. Carmon and H. P. Bechtoldt, “Dominance of the Right Cerebral Hemisphere for Stereopsis,” Neuropsychologia 7 (1969), 29–39; A. Carmon and A. L. Benton, “Tactile Perception of Direction and Number in Patients with Unilateral Cerebral Disease,” Neurology 19 (1969), 525–532; H. D. Cohen, R. C. Rosen, and I. Goldstein, “Electroencephalographic Laterality Changes during Human Sexual Orgasm,” Archives of Sexual Behavior 5 (1976), 189–200; S. Corkin, B. Milner, andT. Rasmussen, “Somatosensory Thresholds: Contrasting Effects of Post-Central Gyrus and Posterior Parietal-Lobe Excisions,” Archives of Neurology 23 (1970), 41–58; E. DeRenzi and G. Scotti, “The Influence of Spatial Disorders in Impairing Tactual Discrimination of Shapes,” Cortex 5 (1969), 53–62; J. E. Desmedt, “Active Touch Exploration of Extrapersonal Space Elicits Specific Electrogenesis in the Right Cerebral Hemisphere of Intact, Right-Handed Man,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 74 (1977), 4037–4040; A. G. Dodds, “Hemispheric Differences in Tactuo-Spatial Processing,” Neuropsychologia 16 (1978), 247–254; J. Horn and R. Reitan, “Effects of Lateralized Cerebral Damage on Contralateral and Ipsilateral Sensorimotor Performance,” Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology 3 (1982), 47–53.

    Google Scholar 

  22. K. Heilman, “Neglect and Related Disorders.” In Clinical Neuropsychology, K. Heilman and E. Valenstein, Eds. (pp. 300–320) (New York: Oxford University Press. 1979); R. Joseph, “Confabulation and Delusional Denial: Frontal Lobe and Lateralized Influences,” Journal of Clinical Psychology 42 (1986), 507–518; Joseph, 1982, 1988, 1990.

    Google Scholar 

  23. H. H. Brownell et al 1986; Cicone et al. 1980; J. W. Dwyer and W. E. Rinn, “The Role of the Right Hemisphere in Contextual Inference,” Neuropsychologia 19 (1981), 479–482; Gardner etal 1983; H. Gardner, P. K. Ling, L. Flamm, and J. Silverman, “Comprehension and Appreciation of Humorous Material Following Brain Damage,” Brain 98 (1975), 399–412.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Joseph, 1988, 1990; P. B. Gorelick and E. D. Ross, ‘Aprosodia,“ Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 37 (1987), 727–737; Heilman et al, 1975; A.M. Mahoney and R. S. Sainsbury, ”Hemispheric Asymmetry in the Perception of Emotional Sounds,“ Brain and Cognition 6 (1987), 216–233; E. Ross, ”The Aprosodias: Functional-Anatomic Organization of the Affective Components of Language in the Right Hemisphere,“ Archives of Neurology 38 (1981), 561–589; Bryden et al, 1982; Carmon and Nachshon, 1973; D. Tucker, ”Affective Discrimination and Evaluation of Affectively Toned Speech,“ Neurology 27 (1977), 947–950; S. Weintraub, M.-M. Mesulam, and L. Kramer, ”Disturbances in Prosody: A Right Hemisphere Contribution to Language,“ Archives of Neurology 38 (1981), 742–744; D. B. Hier, J. Mondlock, and L. R. Caplan, ”Behavioral Abnormalities after Right Hemisphere Stroke,“ Neurology 33 (1983), 337–344; E. Hillbom, ”After-Effects of Brain Injuries,“ Acta Psychiatrica Scartdinavica 142 (Supplement) (1960); R. A. Jack, N. T. Rivers-Bulkeley, and R L. Rabin, ”Secondary Mania as a Presentation of Progressive Dialysis Encephalopathy,“ Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 171 (1983), 193–195; R. C. Jamieson and C. E. Wells, ”Manic Psychosis in a Patient with Multiple Metastic Brain Tumours,“ Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 40 (1979), 280–282; V. C. Jampala and R. Abrams, ”Mania Secondary to Right and Left Hemisphere Damage,“ American Journal of Psychiatry 140 (1983), 1197–1199; W. A. Lishman, ”Brain Damage in Relation to Psychiatry Disability after Head Injury,“ British Journal of Psychiatry 114 (1968), 373–410; M. W. Otto, R. A. Yeo, and M. J. Dougher, ”Right Hemisphere Involvement in Depression: Toward a Neuropsychological Theory of Negative Affective Experiences,“ Biological Psychiatry 22 (1987), 1201–1215; H. A. Sackheim, R. C. Gur, and M. C. Saucy, ”Emotions Are Expressed More Intensely on the Left Side of the Face,“ Science 202 (1979), 424–435.

    Google Scholar 

  25. R. Joseph, “Dual Mental Functioning in a Split Brain Patient,” Journal of Clinical Psychology 44 (1988), 770–779; J. Levy, “Psychological Implications of Bilateral Asymmetry.” In Hemisphere Function in the Human Brain S. Dimond and J. G. Beaumont, Eds. (pp. 121–183) (London: Paul Elek, 1974); J. Levy and C. Trevarthen, “Metacontrol of Hemispheric Functions in Human Split- Brain Patients,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 2 (1976), 299–312.

    Google Scholar 

  26. D. Falk, “Brain Lateralization in Primates and Its Evolution in Hominids,” Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 30 (1987), 107–125; D. Falk, “Hominid Paleoneurology,” Annual Review of Anthropology 30 (1987b), 107–125.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Joseph, 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  28. P. F. MacNeilage, M. G. Studdert-Kennedy, and B. Lindblom, “Functional Precursors to Language and Its Lateralization,” American Journal of Physiology 246 (1984), 187–201.

    Google Scholar 

  29. L. Leroi-Gourhan, Treasures of Prehistoric Art (New York: Abrams, 1960); J. Maringer and H.-G. Bandi, Art in the Ice Age (New York: Praeger, 1952); M. H. Woolpoff, Paleoanthropology (New York: Knopf, 1980 ); T. Prideaux, Cro- Magnon Man ( New York: Time-Life, 1973 ).

    Google Scholar 

  30. Falk, 1987a, b.

    Google Scholar 

  31. R. L. Holloway, “Human Paleontological Evidence to Language Behavior,” Human Neurobiology 2 (1983a), 105–114; R. L. Holloway, “Human Brain Evolution: A Search for Units, Models, and Synthesis,” Canadian Journal of Anthropology 58 (1983b), 1101–1110.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Ibid.

    Google Scholar 

  33. L. R. Binsford, In Pursuit of the Past: Decoding the Archaeological Records ( London: Thames and Hudson, 1983 ).

    Google Scholar 

  34. I. Davidson and Noble, “The Archaeology of Perception: Traces of Depiction and Language,” Current Anthropology 30 (1989), 125–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Prideaux, 1973; Bandi, 1961.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Ibid.; J. Halverson, ‘Art for Art’s Sake in the Paleolithic,“ Current Anthropology 28 (1987), 63–71; P. Mellars, ”The Character of the Middle-Upper Paleolithic Transition in Southwest France.“ In The Explanation of Culture Change, C. Renfrew, Ed. (pp. 255–276) (London: Duckworth, 1973); R. White, ”Rethinking the Middle/Upper Paleolithic Transition,“ Current Anthropology 23 (1982), 169–192; Binsford, 1982, 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Ibid.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1992 R. Joseph

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Joseph, R. (1992). Freud, Jung, and the Evolution and Duality of the Mind and Brain. In: The Right Brain and the Unconscious. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-5996-6_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-5996-6_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-306-44330-5

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-5996-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics