Skip to main content

Language and Cognition in Products Liability

  • Chapter
Language in the Judicial Process

Part of the book series: Law, Society and Policy ((LSPO,volume 5))

Abstract

It is no secret that the last several years have seen a marked increase in the degree of interest and involvement in the legal process on the part of cognitive psychologists. This change is reflected in the creation of a new division of the American Psychological Association devoted entirely to the interface of psychology and law, in an increase in publication of relevant books and articles, and in the appearance of the one clear sign that a subdiscipline has arrived: competition among publishers in soliciting and marketing textbooks designed specifically for courses in psychology and the law. I think it is safe to say, although I am quite sure that the legal profession is not yet entirely comfortable with the prospect, that the mutual influence of the psychological and legal professions upon each other can only grow. In this chapter, I would like to consider some ways in which practitioners of one “psychology and...” hybrid (psychology and language) might contribute to the growth and development of another (psychology and law).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Barclay, J., Bransford, J., Franks, J., McCarrell, N., & Nitch, K. (1974). Comprehension and semantic flexibility. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 471–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D. (1965). The atomization of meaning. Language, 39, 170–210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J., & Johnson, M. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J., & McCarrell, N. (1974). A sketch of a cognitive approach to comprehension: Some thoughts about understanding what it means to comprehend. In W. Weimer & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic process (pp. 189–229). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1965). Social psychology. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. (1976). Frame semantics and the nature of language. In S. Harnad, H. Steklis, & J. Lancaster (Eds.), Origins and evolution of language and speech (pp. 20–32). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, B. (1979). The interpretation of novel metaphors. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 172–185). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harré, R., & Secord, P. (1972). The explanation of social behavior. London: Basil Blackwell & Mott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time (J. Macquarrie & E. Robinson trans.). New York: Harper & Row. (Original work published 1927)

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. (1980). Can we have a fruitful cognitive psychology? Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 28, 211–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M., & Henley, T. (1988). Something old, something new, something borrowed, something true. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 3, 233–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M., & Malgady, R. (1980). Toward a perceptual theory of metaphoric comprehension. In R. Honeck & R. Hoffman (Eds.), Cognition and figurative language (pp. 259–282). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M., Bransford, J., & Solomon, S. (1973). Memory for tacit implications of sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 98, 203–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, J., & Fodor, J. (1963). The structure of a semantic theory. Language, 39, 170–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Loftus, E. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCloskey, M., & Egeth, H. (1983). Eyewitness identification: What can a psychologist tell a jury? American Psychologist, 38, 550–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Minsky, M. (1987). The society of mind. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. (1973). You can’t play twenty questions with nature and win. In W. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 283–308). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noel, D., & Phillips, J. (1981). Products liability (2nd ed.). St. Paul: West.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prosser, W (1971). Handbook of the law of torts (4th ed.). St. Paul: West.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, M. (1979). The conduit metaphor—A case of frame conflict in our language about language. In Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 284–324). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. (1980). Psychology and law. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1968). Words, meanings and messages. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1974). On message structure. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1977). Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannen, D. (1984). Conversational style: Analyzing talk among friends. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verbrugge, R., & McCarrell, N. (1977). Metaphoric comprehension: Studies in reminding and resembling. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 494–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winograd, T. (1981). What does it mean to understand language. In D. Norman (Ed.), Perspectives on cognitive science (pp. 231–264). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winograd, T., & Flores, F. (1986). Understanding computers and cognition. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

7. Cases Cited

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1990 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Johnson, M.G. (1990). Language and Cognition in Products Liability. In: Levi, J.N., Walker, A.G. (eds) Language in the Judicial Process. Law, Society and Policy, vol 5. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-3719-3_10

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-3719-3_10

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-3721-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-3719-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics