Advertisement

The Importance of Mental Imagery in Map Reading

  • Laura L. Snodgrass

Abstract

How do we relate what we see in a map to the world around us? A map provides a bird’s eye view of an extended area with the spatial relationships presented in terms of two-dimensional euclidean geometry. As we look out into the world, we are presented with a horizontal or straight-on view of a limited area -- the extended view is usually cut off by buildings or trees or other objects, and the spatial relationships are presented in terms of perspective geometry. I am currently investigating how people translate between the bird’s eye view with two dimensional euclidean geometry and the horizontal or straight-on view with perspective geometry.

Keywords

Spatial Relationship Mental Rotation Spatial Representation Mental Image Mental Imagery 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Cooper, L. (1975). Mental rotation of random two-dimensional shapes.Cognitive Psychology, 7, 20 - 43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Cooper, L., and Podgorny, P. (1976). Mental transformations and visual comparison processes: effects of complexity and similarity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 503 - 514.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cooper, L., and Shepard, R. N. (1973). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Dodds, A. G., Howarth, C. I., and Carter, D. C. (1982). The mental maps of the blind: The role of previous visual experience. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 76 (1), 5 - 12.Google Scholar
  5. Downs, R. M., and Stea, D. (1977). Maps in mind. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  6. Evans, G. W., Marrero, D. G., and Butler, P. A. (1981). Environmental learning and cognitive mapping. Environment and behavior, 13 (1), 83 - 104.Google Scholar
  7. Fletcher, J. F. (1980). Spatial representation in blind children. 1:Development compared to sighted children. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 74 (10), 381 - 385.Google Scholar
  8. Foley, J., and Cohen, A. (1984). Working mental representations of the environment. Environment and Behavior, 16 (6), 713 - 729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Herman, J. F., Chapman, S. P., and Roth, S. F. (1983). Cognitive mapping in blind people: Acquisition of spatial relationship in a large-scale environment. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 77 (4), 161 - 166.Google Scholar
  10. Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and mind. Massachusetts: HarvardUniversity Press.Google Scholar
  11. Levine, M., Jankovic, I. N., and Palij, M. (1982). Principles of spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 111, 157 - 175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Lieblich, I., and Arbib, M. A. (1982). Multiple representations of space underlying behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, S, 627 - 659.Google Scholar
  13. Millar, S (1976). Spatial representation by blind and sighted children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21, 460 - 470.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Perkins, D. N. (1983). Why the human perceiver is a bad machine. In Beck, J., Hope, B., and Rosenfeld, A. (Eds.), Human andmachine vision. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  15. Pinker, S., and Finke, R. A. (1980). Emergent two-dimensional patterns in images rotated in depth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 6 (2), 244 - 264.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Presson, C. C., and Hazelrigg, M. D. (1984). Building spatial representation through primary and secondary learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10 (4), 716 - 722.Google Scholar
  17. Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1979a). The rate of “mental rotation” of images: a test of a holistic analogue hypothesis. Memory and Cognition, 7, 19 - 28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Rosencranz, D., and Suslick, R. (1976). Cognitive models for spatial representations in congenitally blind, adventitiously blind, and sighted subjects. The New Outlook, 70 (5), 188 - 194.Google Scholar
  19. Shepard, R. N., and Hurwitz, S. (1984). Upward direction, mental rotation, and discrimination of left and right turns in maps. Cognition, 18, 161 - 193.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Shepard R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701 - 703.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Strelow, E. R. (1985). What is needed for a theory of mobility: Direct perception and cognitive maps - Lessons from the blind. Psychological Review, 92 (2), 226 - 248.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189 - 208.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Laura L. Snodgrass
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychology DepartmentMuhlenberg CollegeAllentownUSA

Personalised recommendations