Advertisement

The Scientific Credibility of Commonsense Psychology

  • Garth J. O. Fletcher
Part of the Perspectives on Individual Differences book series (PIDF)

Abstract

By the early 1980s, the idea that ordinary human social cognition is rational or scientific in character was under serious attack in personality and social psychology. Personality psychology was in the throes of the debate concerning whether behavior was consistent across situations and, although the jury was still out on the issue, powerful voices and persuasive data suggested that the concept of personality traits was a quaint shibboleth of commonsense psychology. As Nisbett and Ross (1980) put it, “the personality theorists’ (and the layperson’s) conviction that there are strong cross-situational consistencies in behavior may be seen as merely another instance of theory-driven covariation assessments operating in the face of contrary evidence” (p. 112).

Keywords

Social Cognition Scientific Realism Folk Psychology Social Judgment Personality Theory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Allport, G. W. (1966). Traits revisited. American Psychologist, 21, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Armstrong, D. M. (1969). Dispositions are causes. Analysis, 30, 28–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Asch, S., and Zukier, H. (1984). Thinking about persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 1230–1240.Google Scholar
  4. Bhaskar, R. (1978). A realist theory of science ( 2nd ed. ). Sussex: Harvester Press.Google Scholar
  5. Block, J. (1989). Critique of the act frequency approach to personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 234–245.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Brookings, J. B., and Brown, C. E. ( 1988, April). Dimensionality of the attributional complexity scale. Paper presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  7. Buss, D. M., and Craik, K. H. (1983). The act frequency approach to personality. Psychological Review, 90, 105–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Churchland, P. M. (1984). Matter and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Churchland, P. M. (1985). Reduction, qualia, and the direct introspection of brain states. The Journal of Philosophy, 82, 8–28.Google Scholar
  9. Churchland, P. M. (1988). The ontological status of intentional states: Nailing folk psychology to its perch. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 507–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. D’Andrade, R. G. (1974). Memory and the assessment of behavior. In H. M. Blalock (Ed.), Measurement in the social sciences. Chicago: Aldine-Atherton.Google Scholar
  11. Dennett, D. C. (1987). The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  12. Desoto, C. B., Hamilton, M. M., and Taylor, R. B. (1985). Words, people, and implicit personality theory. Social Cognition, 3, 369–382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Devine, P. (1989). Overattribution effect: The role of confidence and attributional complexity. Social Psychology Quarterly, 52, 149–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Einhorn, H. J., and Hogarth, R. M. (1981). Behavioral decision theory: Processes of judgment and choice. Annual Review of Psychology, 32, 53–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Feyerabend, P. K. (1975). Against method. London: New Left Books.Google Scholar
  16. Fiske, S. T., and Taylor S. E. (1984). Social cognition. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Fletcher, G. J. 0. (1984). Psychology and common sense. American Psychologist, 39, 203–213.Google Scholar
  17. Fletcher, G. J. O., and Fincham, F. D. (1991). Attribution processes in close relationships. In G. J. O. Fletcher and F. D. Fincham (Eds.), Cognition in close relationships (pp. 735 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  18. Fletcher, G. J. 0., and Haig, B. (1992). The layperson as “naive scientist”: An appropriate model for social psychology? Unpublished manuscript, University of Canterbury, Christchurch, New Zealand.Google Scholar
  19. Fletcher, G. J. O., Grigg, F., and Bull, V. (1988). The organization and accuracy of personality impressions: Neophytes versus experts in trait attribution. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 17, 68–77.Google Scholar
  20. Fletcher, G. J. O., Reeder, G. D., and Bull, V. (1990). Bias and accuracy in attitude attribution: The role of attributional complexity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 275–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Fletcher, G. J. O., Rhodes, G., Rosanowski, J., and Lange, C. (1992). Accuracy and speed of causal processing: Experts versus novices in social judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 320–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Flett, G. L., Pliner, P., and Blankstein, K. R. (1989). Depression and components of attribu- tional complexity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 757–764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Funder, D. C. (1987). Errors and mistakes: Evaluating the accuracy of social judgment. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 75–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Furnham, A. F. (1989). Lay theories: Everyday understanding of problems in the social sciences. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  25. Furnham, A., and Henderson, M. (1983). Lay theories of delinquency. European Journal of Social Psychology, 13, 107–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Gilbert, D. T., Pelham, B. W., and Krull, D. S. (1988). On cognitive busyness: When person perceivers meet persons perceived. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 733–741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Greenwood, J. D. (1989). Explanation and experiment in social psychological science. New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
  29. Griggs, R. A., and Cox, J. R. (1983). The elusive thematic-materials effect in Wason’s selection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35, 519–533.Google Scholar
  30. Hastie, R. (1984). Causes and effects of causal attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 44–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hinz, V. B., Tindale, J. E., Nagao, D. H., Davis, J. H., and Robertson, B. A. (1988). The influence of the accuracy of individuating information on the base rate information in probability judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 24, 127–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hirschberg, N. (1978). A correct treatment of traits. In H. London (Ed.), Personality: A new look at meta-theories (pp. 45–68 ). New York: Halstead Press.Google Scholar
  33. Hooker, C. A. (1987). A realistic theory of science. New York: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  34. Howard, G. S. (1985). The role of values in the science of psychology. American Psychologist, 40, 255–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Kenrick, D. T., and Funder, D. C. (1988). Profiting from controversy: Lessons from the person-situation debate. American Psychologist, 43, 23–34.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Klayman, J., and Ha, Y. W. (1987). Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis testing. Psychological Review, 94, 211–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2nd rev. ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  38. Lepper, M. R., Ross, L., and Lau, R. R. (1986). Persistence in inaccurate beliefs about the self: Perseverance effects in the classroom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 482–491.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Manicas, R T., and Secord, R F. (1983). Implications for psychology of the new philosophy of science. American Psychologist, 38, 399–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Markus, H., and Zajonc, R. B. (1985). The cognitive perspective in social psychology. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., pp. 137230 ). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  41. McCrae, R. R., and Costa, R T. (1985). Updating Norman’s “adequate taxonomy”: Intelligence and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 710–721.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. McMullin, E. (1983). Values in science. In R D. Asquith and T. Nickles (Eds.), Proceedings of the 1982 Philosophy of Science Association (Vol. 2, pp. 3–23 ). East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association.Google Scholar
  43. McMullin, E. (1984). A case for scientific realism. In J. Leplin (Ed.), Scientific realism (pp. 8–40 ). Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  44. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  45. Newman, L. S., and Uleman, J. S. (1989). Spontaneous trait inference. In J. S. Uleman and J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 155–188 ). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  46. Nisbett, R. E., and Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-HallGoogle Scholar
  47. Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson (Original work published 1935 )Google Scholar
  48. Rippere, V. (1977). Commonsense beliefs about depression and antidepressive behaviour: A study of social consensus. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 465–470.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Rosenberg, S. C., Nelson, C., and Vivekananthan, R S. (1968). A multidimensional approach to the structure of personality impressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, 619–626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 10, pp. 173–220 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  51. Ross, M., and Fletcher, G. J. 0. (1985). Attribution and social perception. In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology ( 3rd ed., pp. 73–122 ). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  52. Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Barnes and Noble.Google Scholar
  53. Semin, G. R., and Krahe, B. (1987). Lay conceptions of personality: Eliciting tiers of a scientific conception of personality. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 199–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Showers, C., and Cantor, N. (1985). Social cognition: A look at motivated strategies. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 275–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Shweder, R. A. (1975). How relevant is an individual difference theory of personality? Journal of Personality, 43, 455–485.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B. E., Ketron, J. L., and Bernstein, M. (1981). People’s concep- tion of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 37–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Strube, M. J., Lott, C. L., Le-Xuan-Hy, G. M., Oxenberg, J., and Deichmann, A. K. (1986). Self-evaluation of abilities: Accurate self-assessment versus biased self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 16–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Taylor, S. E., and Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Tetlock, P. (1985). Accountability: A social check on the fundamental attribution error. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Trope, Y. (1979). Uncertainty-reducing properties of achievement tasks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 227–236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Trope, Y. (1980). Self-assessment and task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 201–215.Google Scholar
  62. Weiss, D. S., and Mendelsohn, G. A. (1986). An empirical demonstration of the implausibility of the semantic similarity explanation of how trait ratings are made and what they mean. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 595–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Wiggins, J. S. (1974). In defense of traits. Invited address at the Ninth Annual Symposium on Recent Developments in the Use of the MMPI, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
  64. Zuroff, D. C. (1986). Was Gordon Allport a trait theorist? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 993–1000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1993

Authors and Affiliations

  • Garth J. O. Fletcher
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of CanterburyChristchurch 1New Zealand

Personalised recommendations