Skip to main content

Conspicuousness and Diversity in Animal Signals

  • Chapter
Communication

Part of the book series: Perspectives in Ethology ((PEIE,volume 12))

Abstract

Animal signals that are conspicuous or extravagant are commonly explained as costly advertisements of ‘quality.’ There are, however, many other evolutionary explanations of conspicuousness, most notably those to do with ‘efficacy’ or factors making signals effective at getting their message across to receivers. Efficacy can be a major evolutionary force driving signals toward conspicuousness even if there is no selection pressure to advertise quality. Its effects have been widely underestimated through a failure to understand the variety of different ways in which it can act. Efficacy offers a variety of alternative explanations for the evolution of signals, including extravagant sexual signals and can also explain a further feature of animal signals, namely the diversity between species in the signals they use.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alexander, R. D. (1962). Evolutionary change in cricket acoustical communication. Evolution, 16, 443–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andersson, M. (1982). Sexual selection, natural selection and quality advertisement. Biol. J. Linn. Soc., 1, 375–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arak, A. (1988). Female mate selection in the natterjack toad: active choice or passive attraction? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 22, 317–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arak, A. & Enquist, M. (1993). Hidden preferences and the evolution of signals. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 340, 201–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arak, A. & Enquist, M. (1995). Conflict, receiver, bias and the evolution of signal form. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 349, 337–344.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, G. W. (1977). Modal action patterns. In T. A Sebeok (Ed.), How Animals Communicate (pp.98–134). Bloomington. IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basolo, A. (1990). Female preference predates the evolution of the sword in swordtail fish. Science, 250, 808–810.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Basolo, A. (1995). Phylogenetic evidence for the role of a pre-existing bias in sexual selection. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 259, 307–311.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Butlin, R. (1989). Reinforcement or premating isolation. In D. Otte & J. A. Endler (Eds.), Speciation and its Consequences (pp. 158–179). Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christy, J. (1995). Mimicry, mate choice and the sensory trap hypothesis. Amer. Nat., 146, 171–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, D. L. & Uetz, G. W. (1992). Morph-independent mate selection in a dimorphic jumping spider: demonstration of movement bias in female bias using video-controlled courtship behaviour. Anim. Behav., 43, 247–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cresswell, W. (1994). Song as a pursuit-deterrence signal and its occurrence relative to other anti-predator behaviour of the skylark on attack by merlins. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 34, 217–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cullen, J. M. (1966). Reduction of ambiguity through ritualisation. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 215, 363–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, N. B. & Halliday, T. R. (1978). Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads Bufo bufo. Nature, 274, 683–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, M. S. (1989). The future of ethology: how many legs are we standing on? In P. P. G. Bateson & P. H. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspective in Ethology, 8, 47–83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, M. S. & Guilford, T. (1993). Colour and pattern in relation to sexual and aggressive behaviour in the bluehead wrasse Thalassoma bifasciatum. Behav. Proc., 30, 245–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, M. S. & Guilford, T. (1994). Design of an intention signal in the bluehead wrasse Thalassoma bifasciatum). Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 257, 123–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, M. S. & Guilford, T. Sensory bias and the adaptiveness of female choice. Amer. Nat., in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. & Krebs, J. R. (1978). Animal signals: information or manipulation? In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology: an Evolutionary Approach (pp. 282–309). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, J. A. (1988). Sexual selection and predation risk in guppies. Nature, 232, 593–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Endler, J. A. (1993). Some general comments on the evolution and design of animal communication systems. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 340, 215–225.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgibbon, C. D. & Fanshaw, J. H. (1988). Stotting in Thomson’s gazelles: an honest signal of condiion. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 23, 69–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foster, S. A. (1995). Constraint, adaptation, and opportunism in the design of behavioral phenotypes. In N. S. Thompson (Ed.), Perspectives in Ethology, 11, 61–81. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerhardt, H. C. (1994). The evolution of vocalisation in frogs and toads. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst., 25, 293–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gittleman, J. L. & Harvey, P. H. (1980). Why are distasteful prey not cryptic? Nature, 286, 149–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Godfray, H. C. (1991). Signalling of need by offspring to their parents. Nature, 352, 328–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafen, A. (1990a). Sexual selection unhandicapped by the Fisher process. J. Theoret. Biol., 144, 475–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen, A. (1990b). Biological signals as handicaps. J. Theoret. Biol., 144, 517–546.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, M. D. (1994). Cooperation and conflict in the evolution of signal interactions. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst., 25, 97–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, T. (1992). Predator psychology and the evolution of prey coloration. In M. J. Crawley (Ed.), Natural Enemies: The Population Biology of Predators, Parasites and Diseases (pp. 377–394). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, T. & Dawkins, M. S. (1991). Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Anim. Behav., 42, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, T. & Dawkins, M. S. (1993). Receiver psychology and the design of animal signals. Trends in NeuroScience, 16, 430–436.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, T. & Dawkins, M. S. The efficiency of exaggerated signals. Animal Behaviour, (submitted).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hailman, J. P. (1977). Optical Signals. Animal Communication and Light. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, O. (1989). Amplifiers and the handicap principle: different emphasis. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 235, 383–406.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, O. (1991). Sexual displays as amplifiers: practical examples with an emphasis on feather decorations. Behav. Ecol., 2, 189–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasson, O. (1994). Cheating signals. J. Theoret. Biol., 167, 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hill, G. E. (1994). Geographic variation in male ornamentation and female mate preference in the house finch: a comparative test of models of sexual selection. Behav. Ecol., 5, 64–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hinde, R. A. (1981). Animal signals: ethological and games theory approaches are not incompatible. Anim. Behav., 29, 535–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huxley, J. (1966). A discussion of ritualisation of behaviour in animals and man: introduction. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 251, 247–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, R. A. (1995). Sexual selection, honest advertisement and the handicap principle: reviewing the evidence. Biol. Rev., 70, 1–65.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kirkpatrick, M. & Ryan, M. J. (1991). The evolution of mating preferences and the paradox of the lek. Nature, 350, 33–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konishi, M. (1970). Evolution of design features in the coding of species-specificity. Amer. Zool., 10, 67–72.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (1993). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology, (3rd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, J. R. & Dawkins, R. (1984). Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In J. R. Krebs & N. B. Davies (Eds.), Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. (2nd ed., pp. 380–402). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Löfstedt, C. (1993). Moth pheromone genetics and evolution. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 340, 167–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lythgoe, J. N. (1979). The Ecology of Vision. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Magnus, D. B. E. (1958). Experimental analysis of some overoptimal sign stimuli in the mating behaviour of the fritillary butterfly Argynnis paphia L. Proc. 10th Int. Cong. Ent. Montreal, 2, 405–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manning, A. (1967). An Introduction to Animal Behaviour. London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markl, H. (1985). Manipulation, modulation, information, cognition: some riddles of communication. In B. Holldobler & M. Lindauer (Eds.), Experimental Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology (pp. 163–194). Stuttgart: Gustav Fisher Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marier, P. (1959). Developments in the study of animal communication. In P. R. Bell (Ed.), Darwin’s Biological Work (pp. 150–206). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marier, P. (1961). The filtering of external stimuli during instinctive behaviour. In W. H. Thorpe & O. L. Zangwill (Eds.), Current Problems in Animal Behaviour (pp. 150–166). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marier, P. (1967). Animal communication signals. Science, 157, 769–774.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marier, P. (1968). Visual systems. In T. A. Sebeok (Ed.), Animal Communication (pp. 103–126). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marier, P. & Hamilton, W. J. (1966). Mechanisms of Animal Behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1987). Sexual selection—a classification of models. In J. W. Bradbury & M. Andersson (Eds.), Sexual Selection: Testing the Alternatives (pp. 9–20). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1991). Theories of sexual selection. Trends Ecol. Evol., 6, 146–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1994). Must reliable signals always be costly? Anim. Behav., 47, 1115–1120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. & Harper, D. C. G. (1995). Animal signals: models and terminology. J. Theoret. Biol., 177, 305–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, A. J. (1994). Genetic evidence for the ‘good genes’ process in sexual selection. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 35, 235–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, E. S. (1975). Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. Amer. Nat., 111, 855–869.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicoletto, P. F. (1995). Offspring quality and female choice in the guppy. Anim. Behav., 49, 377–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Owings, D. H. & Hennessy, D. F. (1984). The importance of variation in scuirid visual and vocal communication. In J. O. Murie & G. R. Michener (Eds.), The Biology of G round-Dwelling Sciurids: Annual Cycles, Behavioral Ecology and Sociality (pp. 169–226). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, G. A. (1982). Phenotype-limited evolutionarily stable strategies. In: King’s College Sociobiology Group (Eds.), Current Problems in Sociobiology (pp. 173-202).

    Google Scholar 

  • Parker, G. A. (1983). Mate quality and mating decisions. In P. Bateson (Ed.), Mate Choice (pp. 141–166). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paterson, H. E. H. (1993). The specific-mate recognition system and variation in mobile animals. In P. P. G. Bateson, P. H. Klopfer & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Perspectives in Ethology, 10 (pp. 209–227). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrie, M. (1994). Improved growth and survival of offspring of peacocks with more elaborate trains. Nature, 371, 598–599.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pomiankowski, A., Iwasa, Y. & Nee, S. (1991). The evolution of costly mate preferences 1. Fisher and biased mutation. Evolution, 45, 1422–1430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, H. (1991). Courtship in the water mite Neumania papillator: males capitalise on female adaptations for predation. Anim. Behav, 42, 589–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Real, L. A. (Ed.) (1994). Behavioral Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reeve, H. K. & Sherman, P. W. (1993). Adaptation and the goals of evolutionary research. Q. Rev. Biol., 68, 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenthal, G. et al (in press). Animal Behaviour.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rowland, W. J. (1989). Mate choice and the supernormality effect in female sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 24, 433–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, M. J. & Rand, A. S. (1993). Sexual selection and signal evolution: the ghosts of biases past. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B, 340, 187–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanderson, N. (1989). Can gene flow prevent reinforcement? Evolution, 43, 1223–1235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, D. (1969). Insect olfaction: deciphering system for chemical messages. Science, 163, 1031–1037.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Schuler, W. & Roper, T. J. (1992). Responses to warning coloration in avian predators. In P. J. B. Slater, J. S. Rosenblatt, C. Beer & M. Milinski (Eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior, 21 (pp.111–146). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. N. M. (1974). The food searching behaviour of two European thrushes. II The adaptiveness of the search patterns. Behaviour, 49, 1–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, W. J. (1977). The Behavior of Communicating. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, W. J. (1986). An “informational” perspective on manipulation. In R. W. Mitchell & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit (pp. 71–86). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, N. S. (1986). Deception and the concept of behavioural design. In R. W. Mitchell & N. S. Thompson (Eds.), Deception: Perspectives on Human and Nonhuman Deceit (pp. 53–65). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tinbergen, N. (1952). Derived activities; their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution. Q. Rev. Biol., 27, 1–32.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warner, R. R. (1987). Female choice of sites versus males in a coral reef fish, Thalassoma bifasciatum. Anim. Behav., 35, 1470–1478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (1984). Sexual selection, competitive communication and species-specific signals in insects. In T. Lewis (Ed.), Insect Communication (pp. 281–324). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, R. H. (1983). The evolution of communication: information and manipulation. In T. R. Halliday & P. J. B. Slater (Eds.), Animal Behaviour, Vol: 2. Communication (pp. 156–189). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, R. H. (1994). Errors, exaggeration, and deception in animal communication. In L. A. Real (Ed.), Behavioral Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology (pp. 157–189). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—a selection for a handicap. J. Theoret. Biol., 53, 205–214.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1977). Reliability in communication systems and the evolution of altruism. In B. Stonehouse & C. M. Perrins (Eds.), Evolutionary Ecology (pp. 253–259). London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1991). On the definition of sexual selection, Fisher’s model and the evolution of waste and of signals in general. Anim. Behav., 42, 501–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1997 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Dawkins, M.S., Guilford, T. (1997). Conspicuousness and Diversity in Animal Signals. In: Owings, D.H., Beecher, M.D., Thompson, N.S. (eds) Communication. Perspectives in Ethology, vol 12. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1745-4_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1745-4_3

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4899-1747-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4899-1745-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics