Advertisement

Historical Roots

  • Robert W. Rieber
  • Harold J. Vetter
Part of the Cognition and Language book series (CALS)

Abstract

Curiosity about the relation between language and thought is of long standing, going back to the earliest philosophical speculations. Language was of interest to the pre-Aristotelian philosophers because it distinguished human beings from other creatures and appeared intrinsically linked to every aspect of being human: the physical, intellectual, and spiritual self. Pre-Aristotelian philosophers were the first systematic investigators of language, speculating on its origin, the relationship between words and their meanings, and the applications of principles of logic to grammar. It was they who were to lay the foundations of classical European grammar.

Keywords

Verbal Behavior Free Speech Grammatical Category Sound Production Historical Root 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Balken E. R., & Masserman, J. H. (1940). The language of phantasy: III. The language of the phantasies of patients with conversion hysteria, anxiety state, and obsessive-compulsive neuroses. Journal of Psychology, 10, 756–786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Benton, A. L., Hartman, C. H., & Sarason, I. G. (1955). Some relations between speech behavior and anxiety level. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51, 295–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bloomfield, L. (1955). Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  4. Brengelmann, J. C. (1960). Expressive movements and abnormal behavior. In H. J. Eysenck (Ed.), Handbook of abnormal psychology. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
  5. Busemann, A. (1925). Die Sprache der Jugend als Ausdruck der Entwicklungsrhythmik. Jena: Fisher.Google Scholar
  6. Busemann, A. (1926). “Über typische und phäsische Unterschiede der kategorialen Sprachform. Zeitschrift pädagogischen Psychologie, 27, 415–420.Google Scholar
  7. Conrad, D. C., & Conrad, K. (1956). The use of personal pronouns as categories for studying small group interaction. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 277–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chomsky N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  9. Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Verbal behavior by B. F. Skinner. Language, 35, 26–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  11. Chomsky, N. (1972). Linguistic contributions to the study of mind: Past. In N. Chomsky (Ed.), Language and mind (enlarged ed., pp. 1–23). San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
  12. Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  13. Dalgarno, G. (1661). Ars Signorum, vulgo. Character universalis et lingua philosophica. London.Google Scholar
  14. Dalgarno, G. (1680). Didascalophus or The deaf and dumb man’s tutor to which is added a discourse on the nature and number of double consonants. Oxford.Google Scholar
  15. Eisenson, J. (1938). The psychology of speech. New York: Crofts.Google Scholar
  16. Fairbanks, H. (1944). Studies in language behavior: II. The quantitative differentiation of samples of spoken language. Psychological Monographs, 56, 41–74.Google Scholar
  17. Fodor, J. A. (1987). Modules, frames, fridgeons, sleeping dogs, and the music of the spheres. In J. L. Garfield (Ed.), Modularity in knowledge representation and natural language understanding (Chap. 1). Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
  18. Frazier, L. (1988). Grammar and language processing. In F. Newmeyer (Ed.), A survey of linguistic science (Vol. 2, pp. 15–34). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into the human faculty and its development. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Goldman-Eisler, F. A. (1943). A study of individual differences and of interaction in the behavior of some aspects of language interviews. Journal of Mental Science, 100, 97–117.Google Scholar
  21. Goldstein, K. (1948). Language and language disorders. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
  22. Greene, J. (1972). Psycholinguistics: Chomsky and psychology. Baltimore: Penguin Education.Google Scholar
  23. Holder, W. (1669). Elements of speech: An essay of inquiry into natural production of letters: with an appendix concerning persons deaf and dumb. London.Google Scholar
  24. Jackson, H. (1879). An affectation of speech from views of the brain. Brain, 1, 304–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Johnson, W. (1944). Studies in language behavior: I. A program of research. Psychological Monographs, 56, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Laffal, J. (1963). The use of contextual associates in the analysis of free speech. Journal of General Psychology, 69, 51–64.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Laffal, J. (1965a). Pauses in the speech of a schizophrenic patient. Journal of General Psychology, 73, 299–305.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Laffal, J. (1965b). Pathological and normal language. New York: Atherton.Google Scholar
  29. Liebmann, A., & Edel, M. (1903). Die Sprache der Geisteskranken nach stenographischen Aufzeichnungen. Halle a.S.: Marhold.Google Scholar
  30. Lorenz, M., & Cobb, S. (1953). Language behavior in psychoneurotic patients. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 69, 684–694.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lyons, J. (1970). Chomsky. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
  32. Maher, B. A. (1966). Principles of psychopathology. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  33. Mann, M. B. (1944). Studies in language behavior: III. The quantitative differentiation of samples of written language. Psychological Monographs, 56, 41–74.Google Scholar
  34. Marx, O. (1967). The history of the biological basis of language. In E. H. Lenneberg (Ed.), Biological foundations of language (pp. 340–359). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  35. Miller, G. (1962). Some psychological studies of grammar. American Psychologist, 17, 748–762.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Miller, G., & Selfridge, J. A. (1950). Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material. American Journal of Psychology, 63, 176–185.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Müller, F. M. (1887). Science of thought. London: Longman’s Green.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Müller, F. M. (1890). Three lectures on the science of language. Chicago: Open Court.Google Scholar
  39. Müller, F. M. (1907). Three introductory lectures on the science of thought. Chicago: Open Court.Google Scholar
  40. Newman, S., & Mather, V. G. (1938). Analysis of spoken language of patients with affective disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 94, 913–942.Google Scholar
  41. Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  42. Preyer, W. (1894). The mind of the child. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
  43. Raven, C. J. (1958). Verbal dysfunctions in mental illness. Language and Speech, 1, 218–225.Google Scholar
  44. Rieber, R. W. (Ed.) (1976). The neuropsychology of language. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  45. Rochester, S., & Martin, J. R. (1979). Crazy talk: A study of the discourse of schizophrenic patients. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Sanford, F. H. (1942). Speech and personality. Psychological Bulletin, 39, 811–845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Saporta, S. (1961). Psycholinguistics: A book of readings. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  48. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Southard, E. (1916a). On the application of grammatical categories to the analysis of delusions. Philosophical Review, 25, 424–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Southard, E. (1916b). On descriptive analysis of manifest delusions from the subject’s point of view. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 2, 189–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Taylor, J. A. (1953). A personality scale of manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 285–290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wallis, J. (1653). Grammaticae linguae anglicanae cui praefigitur de longue la sive sonorum formatione. Tractatus grammaticus physicus. Oxford.Google Scholar
  53. Wallis, J. (1670). A letter of Doctor J. Wallis to Robert Boyle, Esq. Philosophical Transactions, 5(61), 97–1087.Google Scholar
  54. Wallis J. (1698). A letter of Dr. John Wallis to Mr. Thomas Beverly concerning his methods for instructing persons deaf and dumb. Philosophical Transactions, 30(245), 60–353.Google Scholar
  55. Weckowitz, T. E., & Blewett, D. B. (1959). Size constancy and abstract thinking in schizophrenia. Journal of Mental Science, 105, 909–934.Google Scholar
  56. White, W. A. (1926). The language of schizophrenia. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 16, 395–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. White, W A. (1930). The language of the psychoses. American Journal of Psychiatry, 9, 697–718.Google Scholar
  58. Wyllie, S. (1895). Disorders of speech. London: Oliver & Boyd.Google Scholar
  59. Yongue, V. A model and an hypothesis for language structure. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 104, 444-466.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert W. Rieber
    • 1
  • Harold J. Vetter
    • 2
  1. 1.John Jay College of Criminal Justice and Graduate CenterCity University of New YorkNew YorkUSA
  2. 2.University of South FloridaTampaUSA

Personalised recommendations