Advertisement

Human Organic Memory Disorders: Problems and Interpretations

  • Andrew Mayes

Abstract

Whereas it is known that verbal abilities such as reading and writing break down in several different ways following brain lesions it is commonly believed that multiple specific disorders of memory have not yet been identified. This erroneous impression, which is partly based on the false equation of organic amnesia with all memory disorders, is also associated with the view that the neuropsychology of memory is in a more primitive state than that of language. Such a conclusion cannot be supported for two reasons. First, as will be described in this chapter, there are several kinds of memory disorder that are distinct from organic amnesia and even organic amnesia may comprise more than one kind of functional impairment. Second, there are reasons for thinking that memory disorders will not fractionate in the same way as those of, for example, language.

Keywords

Episodic Memory Semantic Memory Implicit Memory Explicit Memory Retrograde Amnesia 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Albert, M.S., Butters, N. and Brandt, J. (1981). Patterns of remote memory in amnesic and demented patients. Archives of Neurology, 38, 395-500.Google Scholar
  2. Allport, D.A. (1985). Distributed memory modular subsystems and dysphasia. in: S. Newman and R. Epstein (eds.). Current Perspectives in Dysphasia. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
  3. Baddeley, A.D. and Hitch, G. (1974). Working Memory. in: G.H. Bower (ed.). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol.8), New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  4. Bauer, R.M. (1984). Autonomic recognition of names and faces in prosopagnosia: neuropsychological application of a guilty knowledge test. Neuropsychologia, 22, 457-469.Google Scholar
  5. Cohen, N.J. (1984). Preserved learning capacity in amnesia; Evidence for multiple memory systems. in: L.R. Squire and N. Butters (eds.). Neuropsychology of Memory. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  6. Cohen, N.J. and Squire, L.R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skills in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207-210.Google Scholar
  7. Davidoff, J. and Ostergaard, A.L. (1984). Colour anomia resulting from weakened short-term colour memory. Brain, 107, 415-430.Google Scholar
  8. De Renzi, E. and Nichelli, P. (1975). Verbal and non-verbal short-term memory impairment following hemispheric damage. Cortex, 11, 341-354.Google Scholar
  9. Freedman, M. and Cermak, L.S. (1936). Semantic encoding deficits in frontal lobe disease and amnesia. Brain and Cognition, 5, 108-114.Google Scholar
  10. Friedrich, F.I., Glenn, C.G. and Marin, 0.S.M. (1984). Interruption of phonological coding in conduction aphasia. Brain and Language, 22, 288-291.Google Scholar
  11. Graf, P. and Schacter, D.L. (1985). Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 501-508.Google Scholar
  12. Graf, P., Squire, L.R. and Handler, G. (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 164-178.Google Scholar
  13. Hasher, L.D. and Zacks, R.T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356-388.Google Scholar
  14. Heindel, W.C., Butters, N. and Salmon, D. (1987). Impaired motor skill learning associated with neostriatal lesions. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 18.Google Scholar
  15. Hirst, W (1985). Use of mnemonic in patients with frontal lobe damage.Google Scholar
  16. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 7, 175. Huppert, F.A. and Piercy, M. (1979). Normal and Abnormal forgetting in organic amnesia: effect of locus of lesion. Cortex, 15, 385-390.Google Scholar
  17. Kapur, N. (1985). Double dissociation between perseveration in memory and problem solving tasks. Cortex, 21, 461-465.Google Scholar
  18. Kapur, K., Heath, P., Meudell, P. and Kennedy, P. (1986). Amnesia can facilitate memory performance: evidence from a patient with dissociated retrograde amnesia. Neuropsychologia, 24, 215-221.Google Scholar
  19. Kinsbourne, M. (1972). Behavioural analysis of the repetition deficit in conduction aphasia. Neurology, 22, 1126-1132.Google Scholar
  20. Martone, M., Butters, N., Payne, M., Becker, J.T. and Sax, D.S. (1984). Dissociations between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and dementia. Archives of Neurology, 41, 965-970.Google Scholar
  21. Mayes, A.R. (1986). Learning and memory disorders and their assessment. Neuropsychologia, 24, 25-39.Google Scholar
  22. Mayes, A.R. (1987). Human organic memory disorders. in: H. Beloff and A. Colman (eds.). Psychology Survey (Vol.6). Leicester: BPS Publications.Google Scholar
  23. Mayes, A.R. (1988). Recent developments in amnesia research. in: M.M. Gruneberg, P.E. Morris and Oborne, D.J. (eds.). Practical Aspects of Memory (Vol.2). Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
  24. Mayes, A.R. and Meudell, P.R. (1983). Amnesia in humans and other animals. in: A.R. Mayes (eds.). Memory in Animals and Humans. Wokingham: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
  25. Mayes, A.R., Meudell, P.R. and Pickering, A. (1985). Is organic amnesia caused by a selective deficit in remembering contextual information. Cortex, 21, 167-202.Google Scholar
  26. Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the location of psychological processes in man. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272-277.Google Scholar
  27. Milner, B., Petrides, M. and Smith, M.L. (1985). Frontal lobes and the temporal organization of memory. Human Neurobiology, 4, 137-142.Google Scholar
  28. Mishkin, M. (1982). A memory system in the monkey. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 298B, 85-95.Google Scholar
  29. Mishkin, M. and Peters, H.L. (1984). Memories and habits: Some implications for the analysis of learning and retention. in: L.R. Squire and N. Butters (eds.). Neuropsychology of Memory. New York: Guildford.Google Scholar
  30. Parkin, A.J. and Leng, N.R.C. (1987). Comparative studies of human amnesia. in: H. Markowitsch (ed.). Information Processing and the Brain. Toronto: Hans Huber.Google Scholar
  31. Saint-Cyr, J.A., Taylor, A.E. and De Lang, A.E. (1987). Procedural learning impairment in basal ganglia disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9, 280.Google Scholar
  32. Shallice, T. (1986). Impairments of semantic processing: multiple dissociations. in: M. Coltheart, R., Job and G. Sartori, (eds.), The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Language. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  33. Shallice, T. and Warrington, E.K. (1979). Auditory verbal short term memory impairment and conduction aphasia. Brain and Language, 4, 479-491.Google Scholar
  34. Shimamura, A.P., Salmon, D.P., Squire, L.R. and Butters, N. (1987). Memory dysfunction unique to Alzheimer's disease: impairment in word priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9,20.Google Scholar
  35. Signoret, J.L. and Lhermitte, F. (1976). The amnesic syndromes and the encoding process. in: M.R. Rozenzweig and E.L. Bennett (eds.). Neural Mechanisms of Learning and Memory. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT press.Google Scholar
  36. Squire, L.R. (1981). Two forms of amnesia: An analysis of forgetting. Journal of Neuroscience, 1, 635-640.Google Scholar
  37. Squire, L.R. (1987). Memory and the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Starr, A. and Barrett, G. (1987). Disordered auditory short-term memory in man and event related potentials. Brain, 110, 935-959.Google Scholar
  39. Stuss, D.T. and Benson, D.F. (1984). Neuropsychological studies of the frontal lobes. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 3-28.Google Scholar
  40. Thompson, R.E. (1986). The Neurobiology of learning and memory. Science, 233, 941-947.Google Scholar
  41. Tranel, D. and Damasio, A.R. (1985). Knowledge without awareness: An autonomic index of facial recognition. Science, 228, 1453-1454.Google Scholar
  42. Vallar, G. and Baddeley, A.D. (1984). Fractionation of working memory: neuropsychological evidence for a phonological short-term store. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 23, 151-161.Google Scholar
  43. Vallar, G. and Cappa, S.F. (1987). Articulation and verbal short-term memory: evidence from anarthria. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 4, 55-78.Google Scholar
  44. Warrington, E.K. (1982). The fractionation of arithmetical skills: a single case study. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34A, 31-52.Google Scholar
  45. Warrington, E.K. and Rabin, P. (1971). Visual span of apprehension in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 423-431.Google Scholar
  46. Weiskrantz, L. and Warrington, E.K. (1979). Conditioning in amnesic patients. Neuropsychologia, 17, 187-194.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • Andrew Mayes

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations