Cognitive Therapy and Cognitive Science

  • D. J. Tataryn
  • L. Nadel
  • W. J. Jacobs

Abstract

The doctrine of free will supposes that human behavior is the result of rational deliberation and conscious choice. Two recently formulated doctrines—psychoanalysis and behaviorism—that disavow free will for rather different reasons, disagree about what should be put in its place. Cognitive science, the modern study of the mind, offers yet another view on where our behavior comes from and what has to be done to effectively modify it. At stake in all this are matters absolutely central to our understanding of what it is to be human: to think, to feel and to act. The basic question seems to be: What are the causes of our behavior? Other questions arise immediately: Are we, or can we be, consciously aware of these causes? If not, why not? If so, how, and to what avail? The debates among proponents of the various views just noted are more than dry academic affairs; each view suggests a rather different approach to the treatment of people with clinical disorders. This chapter will consider first, and rather briefly, the nature of cognitive therapy and then the emergence and evolution of cognitive science. We will see that the two domains have converged on similar views of cognition and that fruitful collaboration is beginning.

Keywords

Depression Amid Hull Stake Metaphor 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adler, A. (1927). The practice and theory of individual psychology. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, J. R. (1976). Language, memory and thought. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  3. Bachevalier, J., and Mishkin, M. (1984). An early and a late developing system for learning and retention in infant monkeys. Behavioral Neuroscience, 98, 770–778.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
  5. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Beck, A. T. (1963). Thinking and depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 9, 324–333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beck, A. T. (1972). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
  8. Beck, A. T., and Rush, A. J. (1978). Cognitive approaches to depression and suicide. In G. Serban (Ed.). Cognitive defects in development of mental illness (pp. 235–257 ). New York: Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
  9. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., and Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  10. Churchland, P. S. (1986). Neurophilosophy: Toward a unified science of the mind/brain. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
  11. Cohen, N. J., and Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing skill in amnesia. Dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207–209.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Corkin, S. (1968). Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal excision. Neuropsychologia, 6, 255–265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Corteen, R. S., and Wood, B. (1972). Autonomic responses to shock associated words in an unattended channel. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 94, 308–318.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Courts, F. A. (1942). Relation between muscular tension and performance. Psychological Bulletin, 39, 347–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cowey, A. (1961). Perimetry in monkeys. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
  16. Cowey, A., and Weiskrantz, L. (1963). A perimetric study of visual field defects in monkeys. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15, 91–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dennett, D. (1987). Invited address, Philosophy and Psychology Society Meetings, San Diego. Dubois, P. (1906). The influence of the mind on the body. New York: Funk and Wagnalls.Google Scholar
  18. Dubois, P. (1908). The psychic treatment of nervous disorders. New York: Funk and Wagnalls.Google Scholar
  19. Ellis, A. (1958). Rational psychotherapy. Journal of General Psychology, 59, 35–49.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart.Google Scholar
  21. Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  22. Frankl, V. E. (1965). The doctor and the soul. New York: Alfred A. Knopf, Inc.Google Scholar
  23. Frankl, V. E. (1969). The will to meaning. New York: New American Library.Google Scholar
  24. Goldstein, H. (1982). Cognitive therapies: A comparison of phenomenological and mediational models and their origins. The Journal of Mind and Behavior 3, 1–16.Google Scholar
  25. Guidano, V. F., and Liotti, G. (1982). Cognitive processes and emotional disorders. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  26. Graf, P., Squire, L. R., and Mandler, G. (1984). The information that amnesic patients do not forget. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 10, 164–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Guidano, V. F., and Liotti, G. (1985). A constructivistic foundation for cognitive therapy. In M. J. Mahoney and A. Freeman (Eds.), Cognition and psychotherapy (pp. 101–142). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Humphrey, N. K. (1974). Vision in a monkey without striate cortex: A case study. Perception, 3, 241–255.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Humphrey, N. K., and Weiskrantz, L. (1967). Vision in monkeys after removal of the striate cortex. Nature, 215, 595–597.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Jacobs, W. J., and Harris, C. (1985). Escape responding in a shuttle-box. Learning and Motivation, 16, 334–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jacobs, W. J., and Nadel, L. (1985). Stress-induced recovery of fears and phobias. Psychological Review, 92, 512–531.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Kihlstrom, J. F. (1987). The cognitive unconscious. Science, 237, 1445–1452.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kihlstrom, J. F., and Evans, F. J. (1979). Memory retrieval processes in posthypnotic amnesia. In J. F. Kihlstrom and F. J. Evans (Eds.), Functional disorders of memory (pp. 179–218 ). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  34. Kuiper, N. A., and MacDonald, M. R. (1983). Reason, emotion and cognitive therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 3, 297–316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Lazarus, R. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. American Psychologist, 37, 1019–1024.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Mahoney, M. J. (1974). Cognition and behavior modification. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.Google Scholar
  37. Mahoney, M. J. (1984). Behaviorism, cognitivism, and human change processes. In M. A. Reda and M. J. Mahoney (Eds.), Cognitive psychotherapies: Recent developments in theory, research and practice. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.Google Scholar
  38. Marcel, A. J. (1983). Conscious and unconscious perception: Experiments on visual masking and word recognition. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 197–237.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Marshall, J. C. (1987). Is seeing believing? Nature, 325, 583–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Martin, D. G., Stambrook, M., Tataryn, D. J., and Beihl, H. (1984). Conditioning in the unattended left ear. International Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 95–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. McClelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E. (and the PDP Research Group). (1986). Parallel distributed processing. Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 2: Psychological and biological models. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
  42. McEwen, B. S., De Kloet, E. R., and Rostene, W. (1986). Adrenal steroid receptors and actions in the nervous system. Phsyiological Reviews, 66, 1121–1188.Google Scholar
  43. Meichenbaum, D. (1977). Cognitive-behavior modification: An integrative approach. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  44. Meichenbaum, D., and Gilmore, B. (1984). The nature of unconscious processes: A cognitive-behavioral perspective. In K. S. Bowers and D. Meichenbaum (Eds.), The unconscious reconsidered (pp. 273–298 ). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  45. Miller, L. (1986). ‘Narrow localizationism’ in psychiatric neuropsychology. Psychological Medicine, 16, 729–734.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mishkin, M. (1982). A memory system in the monkey. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London [Biology], 298, 85–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Nadel, L., and Wexler, K. (1984). Neurobiology, representations and memory. In G. Lynch, J. L. McGaugh, and N. Weinberger (Eds.), The neurobiology of learning and memory (pp. 125–134 ). New York: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  48. Nadel, L., and Zola-Morgan, S. (1984). Infantile amnesia: A neurobiological perspective. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Infant memory (pp. 145–172 ). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. O’Keefe, J., and Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: The Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  50. Paillard, J., Michel, F., and Stelmach, G. (1983). Localization without content: A tactile analogue of “blind sight”. Archives of Neurology, 40, 548–551.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Pinker, S., and Prince, A. (1988). On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition. Cognition, 28, 73–193.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Rescorla, R. A. (1969). Conditioned inhibition of fear. In N. J. Mackintosh and W. K. Honig (Eds.), Fundamental issues in associative learning (pp. 65–89 ). Halifax: Dalhousie University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Rescorla, R. A. (1980). Pavlovian second-order conditioning: Studies in associative learning. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  54. Rescorla, R. A. (1985). Inhibition and facilitation. In R. R. Miller and N. E. Spear (Eds.), Information processing in animals: Conditioned inhibition (pp. 299–326 ). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  55. Ross, R. T., and LoLordo, V. M. (1987). Devaluation of the relation between Pavlovian occasion-setting and instrumental discriminative stimuli: A blocking analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Animal Behavior Processes, 13, 3–16.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Rumelhart, D. E., McClelland, J. L. (and the PDP Research Group). (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Volume 1: Foundations. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
  57. Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. New York: Barnes and Noble.Google Scholar
  58. Safran, J. D., Vallis, T. M., Segal, Z. V., and Shaw, B. F. (1986). Assessment of core cognitive processes in cognitive therapy. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10, 509–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Schacter, D. L. (1985). Multiple forms of memory in humans and animals. In N. M. Weinberger, J. L. McGaugh, and G. Lynch (Eds.), Memory systems of the brain (pp. 351–379 ). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  60. Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: history and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 501–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Schacter, D. L. (1989). On the relation between memory and consciousness: Dissociable interactions and conscious experience. In H. L. Roediger and F. I. M. Craik (Eds.), Varieties of memory and consciousness: Essays in honor of Endel Tulving (pp. 355–389 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  62. Schacter, D. L., and Graf, P. (1986). Preserved learning in amnesic patients: Perspectives from research on direct priming. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 727–743.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Schacter, D. L., and Moscovitch, M. (1984). Infants, amnesics, and dissociable memory systems. In M. Moscovitch (Ed.), Infant memory (pp. 173–216 ). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Squire, L. R. (1987). Memory and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  66. Tranel, D., and Damasio, A. R. (1985). Knowledge without awareness: An autonomic index of facial recognition by prosopagnosics. Science, 228, 1453–1455.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Warrington, E. K., and Weiskrantz, L. (1968). New method of testing long-term retention with special reference to amnesic patients. Nature, 217, 972–974.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Weiskrantz, L. (1986). Blindsight: A case study and implications. Oxford: The Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
  69. Wilson, W. R. (1979). Feeling more than we can know: Exposure effects without learning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 811–821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • D. J. Tataryn
    • 1
  • L. Nadel
    • 1
  • W. J. Jacobs
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUniversity of ArizonaTucsonUSA
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of LethbridgeLethbridge, AlbertaCanada

Personalised recommendations