Assessment of Visuocognitive Processes

  • Daniel X. Capruso
  • Kerry deS. Hamsher
  • Arthur L. Benton
Part of the Critical Issues in Neuropsychology book series (CINP)


The specialized role of the posterior right hemisphere in visual perception was first advanced by John Hughlings Jackson in 1864. Twelve years later, Jackson (1876/1958) described a patient whose course of illness confirmed his speculations about the lateralization of higher visual function. The clinical onset of illness in Jackson’s patient was marked by a sudden episode of spatial disorientation. Although the patient had resided in the same neighborhood for 30 years, she could not find her way to a nearby park that she had visited frequently. Jackson observed that his patient suffered from what he called “imperception”: the inability to recognize objects, persons, and places. Within months, the patient developed a left hemiplegia and rapidly deteriorated to coma and death. Autopsy revealed a large tumor in the posterior right hemipshere, with two smaller tumors in close proximity.


Facial Recognition Visual Field Defect Line Orientation Aphasic Patient Perceptual Deficit 


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Albert, M. C. (1973). A simple test of visual neglect. Neurology, 23, 658–664.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arena, R., and Gainotti. G. (1978). Constructional apraxia and visuoperceptive disabilities in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions. Cortex, /4, 463–473.Google Scholar
  3. Arrigoni. G., and De Renzi, E. (1964). Constructional apraxia and hemispheric locus of lesion. Cortex, I, 170–197.Google Scholar
  4. Ayres, A. J. (1972). Sensory integration and praxis tests. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Benton, A. L. (1967). Constructional apraxia and the minor hemisphere. Confinia Neurologica, 29. 1–1 h.Google Scholar
  5. Benton, A. L. (1968). Differential behavioral effects of frontal lobe disease. Neuropsychologia, 6, 53–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Benton, A. L. (1969). Disorders of spatial orientation. In P. J. Vinkcn and G. W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology, Vol. 3. Disorders of higher nervous activity (pp. 212–218 ). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  7. Benton, A. L. (1973). Visuoconstructive disability in patients with cerebral disease: Its relationship to side of lesion and aphasic disorder. Documenta Ophthalmologica, 34, 67–76.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Benton, A. L. (1978). The interplay of experimental and brain lesion approaches in train lesion research. In S. Finger (Ed.), Recovers from brain damage: Research and theory (pp. 49–68 ). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Benton, A. L. (1979). Visuoperceptive, visuospatial, and visuoconstructional disorders. In K. M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 186–232 ). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Benton, A. L. (1980). The neuropsychology of facial recognition. American Psychologist, 35, 176186.Google Scholar
  11. Benton, A. L. (1982). Spatial thinking in neurological patients: Historical aspects. In M. Potegal (Ed.), Spatial abilities: Development and physiological foundations (pp. 253–27I ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  12. Benton, A. L. (1985a). Facial recognition 1984. In L. Costa and O. Spreen (lids.), Studies of neuropsy- chology: Selected papers of Arthur Benton (pp. 224–235 ). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Benton, A. L. (1985b). Visuoperceptive, visuospatial, and visuoconstructional disorders. In K. M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology ( 2nd ed., pp. 151–185 ). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Benton, A. L. (1990). Facial recognition 1990. Cortex, 26, 491–499.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Benton, A. L., and Fogel, M. L. (1962). Three-dimensional constructional praxis: A clinical test. Archives of Neurology, 7, 347–354.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Benton, A. L., and Hamsher, K. deS. (1989). Multilingual Aphasia Lxamination ( 2nd ed. ). Iowa City, IA: MA Associates.Google Scholar
  17. Benton. A. L., Hamsher, K. deS., Varney, N. R., and Spreen, O. (1983). Contributions to neurops_rchological assessment: A clinical manual. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  18. Benton, A. L., Hannay, H. J.. and Varney, N. R. (1975). Visual perception of line direction in patients with unilateral brain disease. Neurology, 25. 907–910.Google Scholar
  19. Benton. A. L., and Hécaen, H. (1970). Stereoscopic vision in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Neurology, 20, 1084–1088.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Benton, A. L., and Van Allen, M. W. (1968). Impairment in facial recognition in patients with cerebral disease. Cortex, 4, 344–358.Google Scholar
  21. Benton, A. L., and Van Allen, M. W. (1972). Prosopagnosia and facial recognition. Journal of Neurological.Sciences, 15, 167–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Benton, A. L., Varney, N. R., and Hamsher, K. deS. (1978). Visuospatial judgment: A clinical test. Archives of Neurology. 35, 364–367.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Bisiach, E., and Luzzatti, C. (1978). Unilateral neglect of representational space. Cortex, 14. 129–133.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Bisiach, E., Nichelli, P., and Spinnler, H. (1976). Hemispheric functional asymmetry in visual discrimination between univariate stimuli: An analysis of sensitivity and response criterion. Neuropsvchologia, /4, 3. 35–342.Google Scholar
  25. Bodamer, J. (1947). Die Prosop-Agnosie. Archive fur and Nervenkrankheiten, /79, 6–54. Bodis-Wollner, I., Yahr, M. D., Mylin, L., and Thornton, J. (1982). Dopaminergic deficiency and delayed visual evoked potentials in humans. Annals of Neurology, Il, 478–483.Google Scholar
  26. Boller, F., Passafiume, D.. Keefe, N. C., Rogerse, K., Morrow, L., and Kim, Y. (1984). Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson’s disease: Role of perceptual and motor factors. Archives of Neurology, 41, 485–490.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Brain, W. R. (1941). Visual disorientation with special reference to lesions of the right cerebral hemisphere. Brain, 64, 224–272.Google Scholar
  28. Brun, A., and Englund. E. (1981). Regional pattern of degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease: Neuronal loss and histopathological grading. Histopathology, 5, 549–564.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Capruso, D. X., Hamsher, K. deS., Levin, H. S., and Barr, W. B. (1993). Retrograde memory in patients with unilateral CVA vs. patients with amnesia. Neurology, 43(Suppl. 2 ), A298–299.Google Scholar
  30. Capruso, D. X., and Risser, A. H. (1992). Facial Recognition Test: Identical stimulus matching and transformed stimulus matching. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 41.Google Scholar
  31. Carmon, A., and Bechtoldt, H. P. (1969). Dominance of the right cerebral hemisphere for stereopsis. Neuropsychologia, 7, 29–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Chagas, C., Gattass, R., and Gross, C. (1985). Pattern recognition mechanisms. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  33. Corkin, S. (1979). Hidden-Figures-Test performance: Lasting effects of unilateral penetrating head injury and transient effects of bilateral cingulotomy. Neuropsychologia, 17, 585–605.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Costa, L. D., and Vaughan, H. G. (1962). Performance of patients with lateralized cerebral lesions. L Verbal and perceptual tests. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 134, 162–168.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Cowey, A.. and Gross, C. G. (1970). Effects of foveal prestriate and inferotemporal lesions on visual discrimination by rhesus monkeys. Experimental Brain Research, 11, 128–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Cowey, A., and Wilkinson, F. (1991). The role of the corpus callosum and extra striate visual areas in stereoacuity in the macaque monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 29, 465–479.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Critchley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. New York: Hafner.Google Scholar
  38. Cronin-Golumb, A., Corkin, S., Rizzo, J. F., Cohen, J., Growdon, J. H., and Banks, K. S. (1991). Visual dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: Relation to normal aging. Annals of Neurology. 29, 41–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Damasio, A. R., and Benton, A. L. (1979). Impairment of hand movements under visual guidance. Neurology, 29, 170–178.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Damasio, A. R., and Danìasio. H. (1983). Localization of lesions in achromatopsia and prosopagnosia.Google Scholar
  41. In A. Kertesz (Ed.). Localization in neuropsychology (pp. 417–428). New York: Academic Press. Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., and Chui, H. C. (1980). Neglect following damage to frontal lobe or basal ganglia. Neuropsychologia, 18, 123–132.Google Scholar
  42. Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., and Van Hoesen, G. W. (1982). Prosopagnosia: Anatomic basis and behavioral mechanisms. Neurology, 32, 331–341.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Dec. H. L. (1970). Visuoconstructive and visuoperceptive deficit in patients with unilateral cerebral lesions. Neuropsychologia, 8, 305–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Delis, D. C., Kiefner, M. G., and Fridlund, A. J. (1988). Visuospatial dysfunction following unilateral brain damage: Dissociations in hierarchical and hemispatial analysis. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 421–431.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. De Renzi, E. (1982). Disorders of space exploration and cognition. Chichester, England: Wiley.Google Scholar
  46. De Renzi, E. (1985). Disorders of spatial orientation. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of clinical neurology. Vol. 1(45). Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 405–422 ). North Holland: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  47. De Renzi, E. (1986). Current issues on prosopagnosia. In H. D. Ellis, M. A. Jeeves, F. Newcombe, and A. Young (Eds.), Aspects of face processing (pp. 243–252 ). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., and Scotti, G. (1970). Hemispheric contribution to the exploration of space through the visual and tactile modality. Cortex, 6, 191–203.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. De Renzi, E. Faglioni, P., and Scotti, G. (1971). Judgment of spatial orientation in patients with focal brain damage. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 34, 489–495.Google Scholar
  50. De Renzi, R., Faglioni, P., Scotti, G., and Spinnler, H. (1972). Impairment of color sorting behavior after hemispheric damage: An experimental investigation with the Holmgren skein test. Cortex, 8, 147–163.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., and Spinnler, H. (1968). The performance of patients with unilateral damage on facial recognition tests. Cortex, 4, 17–34.Google Scholar
  52. De Renzi, E., and Spinnler, H. (1966). Visual recognition in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 142, 515–525.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. De Renzi, E., and Spinnler, H. (1967). Impaired performance on color tasks in patients with hemispheric damage. Cortex, 3, 194–216.Google Scholar
  54. Desimone, R., Schein, S. J., Moran, J., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1985). Contour, color, and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex. Vision Research, 25, 441–452.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Deutsch, G., Bourbon, W. T., Papanicolaou, A. C., and Eisenberg, H. M. (1988). Visuospatial tasks compared via regional cerebral blood flow. Neuropsychologia, 26 (3), 445–452.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Duensing, F. (1953). Raumagnostiche und Ideatorisch-Apraktische Störung des gestaltenden Handelns. Deutsche Zeitsschrift für Nervenheilkunde, 170, 72–94.Google Scholar
  57. Eslinger, P. J., and Benton. A. L. (1983). Visuoperceptual performances in aging and dementia: Clinical and theoretical implications. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 5, 213–220.Google Scholar
  58. Eslinger, P. J., Damasio, A. R., Benton, A. L., and Van Allen, M. (1985). Neuropsychological detection of abnormal mental decline in older persons. Journal of the Americn Medical Association, 253, 670–674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Ettlinger, G. (1990). “Object vision” and “spatial vision”: The neuropsychological evidence for the distinction. Cortex -, 26, 319–34I.Google Scholar
  60. Ettlinger, G., Warrington, E.. and Zangwill, O. L. (1957). A further study of visuo-spatial agnosia. Brain, 80, 335–361.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Farah, M. J. (1990). Visual agnosia: Disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  62. Farah, M. J., Wong, A. B., Monheit, M. A., and Morrow. L. A. (1989). Parietal lobe mechanisms of spatial attention: Modality-specific or supramodal? Neuropsychologia, 27 (94), 461–470.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Fontenot, D. J., and Benton, A. L. (1972). Perception of direction in the right and left visual fields. Neuropsychologia, 10, 447–452.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Freedman, L., and Dexter, L. E. (1991). Visuospatial ability in cortical dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 677–690.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Freud, S. (1953). On aphasia. New York: International Universities Press. (Original work published 1891 )Google Scholar
  66. Gainotti, G. (1972). A quantitative study of the “closing-in’’ symptom in normal children and in brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 10, 429–436.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Gainotti, G. (1985). Constructional apraxia. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Handbook of clinical neurology, Vol. 1(45). Clinical neuropsvchologr (pp. 491–506 ). North Holland: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  68. Gainotti, G., Messerli, P., and Tissot, R. (1972). Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebral lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 35, 545–550.Google Scholar
  69. Gainotti, G., Parlato, V., Monteleone, I)., and Carlomagno, S. (1992). Neuropsychological markers of dementia on visual-spatial tasks: A comparison between Alzheimer’s type and vascular forms of dementia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neurops_ychologr, 14, 239–254.Google Scholar
  70. Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Brain, 88, 237–294. Geschwind, N., and Fusillo, M. (1964). Color-naming defects in association with alexia. Transactions on the American Neurological Association, 89, 172–176.Google Scholar
  71. Geschwind, N., and Fusillo, M. (1966). Color-naming defects in association with alexia. Archives of Neurology, 15, 137–146.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Gloning, l., Gloning, K., and Hoff, H. (1968). Neurological symptoms and syndromes in lesions of the occipital lobe and the adjacent areas. Paris: Gauthier-Villars.Google Scholar
  73. Goodglass, H., and Kaplan, E. (1983). The assessment of aphasia and related disorders ( 2nd ed. ). Philadelphia, PA: Lea and Febiger.Google Scholar
  74. Gross, C. G., Desimone, R., Albright, T. D., and Schwartz, E. L. (1985). Inferior temporal cortex and pattern recognition. In Chagas, C., Gattass, R., and Gross, C. (Eds.), Pattern recognition mechanisms (pp. 179–201 ). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  75. Hamsher, K. deS. (1976). Line Cancellation Test. Iowa City: University of Iowa.Google Scholar
  76. Hamsher, K. deS. (1978a). Stereopsis and unilateral brain disease. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 4, 336–343.Google Scholar
  77. Hamsher, K. deS. (1978b). Stereopsis and the perception of anomalous contours. Neuropsvchologia, 16, 453–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Hamsher, K. deS. (1984). Specialized neuropsychological assessment methods. In G. Goldstein and M.Google Scholar
  79. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of psychological assessment (pp. 235–256). New York: Pergamon Press. Hamsher, K. deS. (1991). Intelligence and aphasia. In M. T. Sarno (Ed.), Acquired aphasia (2nd ed..pp. 339–372). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  80. Hamsher, K. deS., Capruso, D. X., and Benton, A. L. (1992). Visuospatial judgment and right hemisphere disease. Cortex, 28, 493–495.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Hamsher, K. deS., and Lee, G. P. (1986). Aproseria: An of a nomen nudem. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Veldhoven, The Netherlands.Google Scholar
  82. Hamsher. K. deS., Levin, H. S., and Benton, A. L. (1979). Facial recognition in patients with focal brain lesions. Archives of Neurology, 36, 837–839.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Hécaen, H., Ajuriaguerra, J., and Massonet, J. (1951). Les troubles visuoconstructive par lésion pariétooccipitale droite. Enrc%phale. 40, 122–179.Google Scholar
  84. Hécaen, H., and Angelergues, R. (1962). Agnosia for faces (prosopagnosia). Archives of Neurology, 7, 92–100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Hécaen, H., Penfield, W., Bertrand, C., and Malmo, R. (1956). The syndrome of apractognosia due toGoogle Scholar
  86. lesions of the minor cerebral hemisphere. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 75,400–434. Heilman, K. M., Bowers, D., Coslett, H. B., and Watson, R. T. (1983). Directional hypokinesia in neglect. Neurology,33(Suppl. 2), 104.Google Scholar
  87. Heilman, K. M., and Valenstein, E. (1972). Frontal lobe neglect in man. Neurology, 22, 660–664. Heilman, K. M.. Watson, R. T., and Valenstein, E. ( 1985 ). Neglect and related disorders. In K. M.Google Scholar
  88. Heilman and E. Valenstein (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (2nd ed., pp. 243–294). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  89. Heilman, K. M., Watson, R. T., Valenstein, E., and Damasio, A. R. (1983). Localization of lesions in neglect. In A. Kertesz (Ed.), Localization in neuropsychology (pp. 471–492 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  90. Hooper, H. E. (1983). Hooper Visual Organization Test. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Huber, S. J., Shuttleworth, E. C.. and Paulson, G. W. (1986). Dementia in Parkinson’s diseae. Archives of Neurology, 43, 987–990.Google Scholar
  91. Iwai, E.. and Mishkin, M. (1968). Two visual foci in the temporal lobe of monkeys. In N. Yoshii and N. A. Buchwald (Eds.), Neurophysiological basis of learning and behavior. Osaka, Japan: Osaka University Press.Google Scholar
  92. Jackson, J. H. (1875/1958). On the anatomical and physiological localization of movements in the brain. In J. Taylor (Ed.), Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson, Vol. 1. On epilepsy and epileptiform convulsion (pp. 37–76). London: Staples Press.Google Scholar
  93. Jackson, J. H. (1876/1958). Case of large cerebral tumor without optic neuritis and with left hemiplegia and imperception. In J. Taylor (Ed.), Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson, Vol. 2. Evolution and dissolution of the nervous system, speech, various papers, addresses, and lectures (pp. 146–152). London: Staples Press.Google Scholar
  94. Jones, B. M. (1971). Verbal and spatial intelligence in short-and long-term alcoholics. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 153, 292–297.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Jones, B. M., and Parsons, O. A. (1972). Specific vs. generalized deficits of abstracting ability in chronic alcoholics. Archives of General Psychiatry, 26, 380–384.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Julesz, B. (1971). Foundations of cyclopean perception. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kanizsa, G. (1986). Subjective contours. In J. M. Wolfe (Ed.), The mind’s eye. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  97. Kaplan, E. (1990). The process approach to neuropsychological assessment of psychiatric patients. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 2(1), 72–87.Google Scholar
  98. Kertesz, A., and Dobrowlski, S. (1981). Right hemisphere deficits: Lesion size and location. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychologv, 3(4), 283–299.Google Scholar
  99. Kinsbourne, M., and Warrington, E. K. (1962). A disorder of simultaneous form perception. Brain, 85, 461–486.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Kinsbourne, M., and Warrington, E. K. (1963). The localizing significance of simultaneous form perception. Brain, 85, 697–702.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Kirk, A., and Kertesz. A. (1989). Hemispheric contributions to drawing. Neuropsychologia, 27(6), 881886.Google Scholar
  102. Kleist, K. (1912). Der Gang und der gegenwärtige Strand der Apraxieforschung. Ergebnisse der Neurologie und Psychiatrie, /, 342–452.Google Scholar
  103. Kleist, K. (1922). Kriegsverletungen des Gehirns in ihrer Bedeutung für die Hirnlokalisation und Hirnpathologie. In O. von Schjerning (Ed.), Handbuch der drt°lichen Erf thrung inn Weltkrieg, Bd. 4. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
  104. Kleist, K. (1934). Gehirnpathologie. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
  105. Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., Blusewicz, M. J., and Preston, K. A. (1991). Visual hierarchical analysis of Block Design configural errors. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, /3, 455465.Google Scholar
  106. Lansdell, H. (1970). Relation of extent of temporal removals to closure and visuomotor factors. Perceptual und Motor Skills, 31, 491–498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Lawler, K. A., and Cowey, A. (1986). The effects of pretectal and superior colliculus lesions on binocular vision. Experimental Brain Research, 63, 402–408.Google Scholar
  108. Lee, G. P.. and Hamsher, G. P. (1988). Neuropsychological findings in toxicometaholic confusional states. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neurops, ychologv, 10, 769–778.Google Scholar
  109. Lee, G. P., Sasanuma, S., Hamsher, K. deS., and Benton. A. L. (1991). Constructional praxis performance of Japanese and American normal and brain-damaged patients. Archives of Clinical Neuro-psychology, 6, 15–25.Google Scholar
  110. Levick, W. (1982). The Judgment of Line Orientation test-A clinical validation. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Newcastle, Australia.Google Scholar
  111. Levin, B. E., Llabre, M. M., Reisman, S., Weiner, W. J., Sanchez-Ramos, J., Singer, C., and Brown, M. C. (1991). Visuospatial impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 41, 365–369.Google Scholar
  112. Levin, B. E., Llabre, M. M., and Weiner. W. J. (1989). Cognitive impairments associated with early Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 39, 557–561.Google Scholar
  113. Levin, H. S.. Grossman, R. G., and Kelly, P. J. (1977). Impairment of facial recognition after closed head injury of varying severity. Corte.v, 13, 119–130.Google Scholar
  114. Lewis, D. A., Campbell, M. J., Terry, R. D.. and Morrison, J. H. (1987). Laminar and regional distributions of neurolibrillary tangles and neuritic plaques in Alzheimer’s disease: A quantitative study of visual and auditory cortices. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 1799–18(18.Google Scholar
  115. Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsvchological assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. Livingstone, M., and Hubel, D. (1987). Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, color, movement, and depth. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 3416–3468.Google Scholar
  116. Livingstone, M., and Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of lorm, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and perception. Science, 240, 740–749.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Luria, A. R., and Tsvetkova, L. D. (1964). The programming of constructive activity in local brain injuries. Neuropsychologia, 2, 95–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Masure, M. C., and Benton, A. L. (1983). Visuospatial performance in left-handed patients with unilateral brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 21, 179–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Masure, M. C., and Tzavaras, A. (1976). Perception de figures entrécroisecs par des sujets atteints de lésions corticales unilatérales. Neuropsychalogoia, /4, 371–374.Google Scholar
  120. Matarazzo, J. D. (1972). Wechsler’s measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence (5th ed.). Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.Google Scholar
  121. Matarazzo, J. D. (1990). Psychological assessment versus psychological testing: Validation from Binet to the school, clinic, and classroom. American Psychologist, 45, 999–1017.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Matarazzo, J. D., and Herman, D. O. (1984). Base-rate data for the WAIS-R: Test-retest reliability and VIQ-PIQ differences. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6, 351–366.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Matthews, C. G. (1992). Truth in labeling: Are we really an international society? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsvchology, 14, 418–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Maunsell, J. H. R. (1987). Psychological evidence for two visual subsystems. In L. M. Vaina (Ed.), Matters of intelligence: Conceptual structures in cognitive neuroscience (pp. 59–87 ). Boston: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Mayer-Gross, W. (1935). Some observations on apraxia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 28, 1203–1212.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  126. McFie. J. (1975). Assessment of organic intellectual impairment. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  127. McFie, J., Piercy, M. F., and Zangwill, O. L. (1950). Visual spatial agnosia associated with lesions of the right hemisphere. Brain, 73, 167–190.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. McFie, J., andZangwill, O. A. (1960). Visual constructive disabilities associated with lesions of the left cerebral hemisphere. Brain, 83, 243–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. McGlynn, S. M., and Schaefer, D. L. (1989). Unawareness of deficits in neuropsychological syndromes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychologv, I I, 143–205.Google Scholar
  130. Meadows, J. C. (1974). The anatomical basis of prosopagnosia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 37, 615–632.Google Scholar
  131. Mehta, Z., Newcombe, F., and Damasio, H. (1987). A left hemisphere contribution to visuospatial processing. Cortex, 23, 447–461.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Mendez, M. F., Mendez, M. A., Martin, R., Smyth, K. A., and Whitehouse, P. J. (1990). Complex visual disturbances in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 40, 439–443.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Milberg, W. P., Hebben, N., and Kaplan, E. (1986). The Boston process approach to neuropsychological assessment. In I. Grant and K. M. Adams (Eds.), Neuropsychological assessment of neurops_vchiatric disorders (pp. 65–86 ). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  134. Mohr, E., Litvan, 1., Williams, J., Fedio, P., and Chase, T. N. (1990). Selective deficits in Alzheimer and Parkinsonian dementia: Visuospatial function. Canadian Journal of Neurological Sciences. /7(3), 292–297.Google Scholar
  135. Mooney, C. M. (1956). Closure with negative after-images under flickering light. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10, 191–199.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  136. Mortimer, J. A., Pirozzolo, F. J., Hansch, E. C., and Webster, D. D. (1982). Relationship of motor symptoms to intellectual deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology, 32, 133–137.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Mountcastle, V. B., Lynch, J. C., Georgopoulos, A., Sakata, H., and Acuna, C. (1975). Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: Command functions for operations within extrapersonal space. Journal of Neurophysiologv, 38, 871–908.Google Scholar
  138. Newcombe, F. (1969). Missile sounds of the brain: A study of psychological deficits. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  139. Newcombe, F. (1985). Neuropsychology qua interface. Journal of Clinical and Neuropsychology. 7, 663–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Newcombe, F.. and Ratcliff, G. (1989). Disorders of visuospatial analysis. In F. Boller and J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsvchology (Vol. 2, pp. 333–356 ). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  141. Newcombe, F.. Ratcliff, G.. and Damasio, H. (1987). Dissociable visual and spatial impairments following right posterior cerebral lesions: Clinical, neuropsychological, and anatomic evidence. Neuropsychologia, 25, 149–161.Google Scholar
  142. Newcombe. F., and Russell, W. R. (1969). Dissociated visual perceptual and spatial deficits in focal lesions of the right hemisphere. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 32. 73–81.Google Scholar
  143. Ober, B. A., Jagust, W. J., Koss, E., Delis, D. C., and Friedland, R. P. (1991). Visuoconstructive performance and regional cerebral glucose metabolism in Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, /3, 752–772.Google Scholar
  144. Parsons, O. A., and Farr, S. P. (1981). The ncuropsychology of alcohol and drug use. In S. B. Filskov and T. J. Boll (Eds.). Handbook of clinical ncuropsychology (pp. 320–365 ). New York: WileyInterscicnce.Google Scholar
  145. Parsons. O. A., and Leber, W. R. (1981). The relationship between cognitive dysfunction and brain damage in alcoholics: Causal, interactive, or epiphenomenal? Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 5, 326–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Paterson, A., and Zangwill, O. L. (1944). Disorders of visual space perception associated with lesions of the right hemisphere. Brain, 67, 331–358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  147. Paterson, A., and Zangwill. O. A. (1945). A case of topographical disorientation associated with unilateral cerebral lesion. Brain, 68, 188–211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  148. Piercy, M. F., and Smyth, V. (1962). Right hemisphere dominance for certain nonverbal intellectual skills. Brain, 85, 775–790.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Poggio, G. F. (1984). The analysis of stereopsis. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 7, 379–412.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  150. Pohl, W. (1973). Dissociation of spatial discrimination deficits following frontal and parietal lesions in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 227–239.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  151. Poppelreuter, W. (1990). Disturbances of lower and higher visual capacities caused by occipital damage. Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1917 )Google Scholar
  152. Price, M. J., Feldman, R. G., Adelberg, D., and Kayne, H. (1992). Abnormalities in color vision and contrast sensitivity in Parkinson’s disease. Neurology. 42, 887–890.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Ptito, A., Zatorre, R. J., Larson, W. L.. and Tosoni, C. (1991). Stereopsis after unilateral temporal lobectomy. Brain, 114, 1323–1333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  154. Quaglino, A., and Borelli, G. (1867). Emiplegia sinistra con amaurosi guarigione-perdita totale della percezione dei colori e della memoria della configurazione degli oggetti. Giornale d’Oftalmologia Italian°, 10, 106–117.Google Scholar
  155. Rizzo, M., and Damasio, H. (1985). Impairment of stereopsis with focal brain lesions. Annals of Neurology, 18, 112.Google Scholar
  156. Roberts, R. J., and Hamsher, K. deS. (1984). Effects of minority status on facial recognition and naming performance. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40, 539–545.Google Scholar
  157. Rosenberger, P. (1974). Discriminative aspects of visual hemi-inattention. Neurology, 24, 17–23. Russo, M., and Vignolo, L. A. (1967). Visual figure-ground discrimination in patients with unilateral cerebral disease. Cortex, 3, 118–127.Google Scholar
  158. Ryan, C. J., and Butters, N. (1982). Cognitive effects in alcohol abuse. In B. Kissin and H. Begleiter (Eds.), Biology of alcoholism, Vol. VI. Biological pathogenesis of alcoholism (pp. 485–538 ). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  159. Schenkenberg, T., Bradford, D. C., and Ajax, E. F. (1980). Line bisection and unilateral visual neglect in patients with neurologic impairment. Neurology, 30, 509–517.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  160. Scotti, G., and Spinnler, H. (1970). Colour imperception in unilateral hemisphere-damaged patients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and P.sychiatrv, 33, 23–28.Google Scholar
  161. Selemon, L. D., and Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1988). Common cortical and suhcortical targets of the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: Relation between intraparietal and principal sulcal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 8, 4049–4068.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  162. Semmes, J.. Weinstein, S., Ghent, L., and Teuber, H. L. (1955). Spatial orientation in man after cerebral injury-I: Analyses by locus of lesion. Journal of Psychology, 39, 227–244.Google Scholar
  163. Sergent, J., Ohta, S., and McDonald, B. (1992). Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing: A positron emission tomography study. Brain, 115, 15–36.Google Scholar
  164. Sergent, J., and Signoret, J. L. (1992). Functional and anatomical decomposition of face processing: Evidence from prosopagnosia and PET study of normal subjects. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B_355, 55–62.Google Scholar
  165. Ska, B., Poissant, A., and Joanette, Y. (1990). Line orientation judgment in normal elderly and subjects with dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 12, 695–702.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Spreen, O., and Benton, A. L. (1969). Neurosensory Center Comprehensive E.ramination for Aphasia. Victoria, B.C.: University of Victoria. Neuropsychology Laboratory.Google Scholar
  167. Spreen, O., and Strauss, E. (19911. A compendium of neuropsychological tests. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  168. Street, R. F. (1931). A gestalt completion test (Contributions to education, No. 481 ). New York: Bureau of Publications, Teacher’s College. Columbia University.Google Scholar
  169. Talland, G. A. (1965). Deranged memory. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  170. Tarter, R., and Edwards, K. (1985). Neuropsychology of alcoholism. In R. Tarter and D. Van Thiel (Eds.), Alcohol and the brain: Chronic effects (pp. 217–242 ). New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  171. Taylor, A. M., and Warrington, E. K. (1973). Visual discrimination in patients with localized brain lesions. Cortex, 9, 82–83.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  172. Teuber, H. L. (1965). Disorders of higher tactile and visual functions. Neuropsychologia, 3, 287–294. Teuber, H. L., Battersby, W. S., and Bender, M. B. (1951). Performance of complex visual tasks after cerebral lesions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 114, 413–429.Google Scholar
  173. Teuber, H. L., Battersby, W. S., and Bender, M. B. (1960). Visual field defects after penetrating missile wounds of the brain. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  174. Thurstone, L. L. (1944). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  175. Trahan, D. E. (1991). Judgment of line orientation in patients with unilateral vascular lesions or severe head trauma. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 23–24.Google Scholar
  176. Ungerleider, L. G., and Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, and R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–585 ). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  177. van Kleeck, M. H. (1989). Hemispheric differences in global versus local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in normal subjects: New data and a meta-analysis of previous studies. Neuropsychologia, 27, 1165–1178.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  178. Varney, N. R. (1982). Colour association and “colour amnesia” in aphasia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 45, 248–252.Google Scholar
  179. Varney, N. R., and Damasio, H. (1986). CT scan correlates of sound recognition defect in aphasia. Cortex, 22, 483–486.Google Scholar
  180. Varney, N. R., and Damasio, H. (1987). Locus of lesion in impaired pantomime recognition. Cortex, 23, 699–703.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  181. Walsh, K. W. (1978). Neuropsychology: A clinical approach. New York: Churchill Livingstone. Walsh, K. W. (1985). Understanding brain damage: A primer of neuropsychological evaluation. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone.Google Scholar
  182. Warrington, E. K. (1982). Neuropsychological studies of object recognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B298, 15–33.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  183. Warrington, E. K., and James, M. (1967). An experimental investigation of facial recognition in patients with unilateral lesions. Cortex, 3. 317–326.Google Scholar
  184. Warrington, E. K., and Rabin, P. (1970). Perceptual matching in patients with cerebral lesions. Neuropsychologia, 8, 475–487.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  185. Wasserstein, J., Zappulla, R., Rosen, J., Gerstman, L., and Rock, D. (1987). In search of closure: Subjective contour illusions, gestalt completion tests, and implications. Brain and Cognition, 6, 1–14.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  186. Watson. R. T., and Heilman, K. M. (1979). Thalamic neglect. Neurology, 29, 690–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  187. Watson, R. T., Heilman, K. M., Cauthen, J. C., and King, F. A. (1973). Neglect after cingulectomy. Neurology, 23, 1003–1007.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  188. Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  189. Whitely, A. M., and Warrington, E. K. (1977). Prosopagnosia: A clinical, psychological, and anatomical study of three patients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 40, 395–403.Google Scholar
  190. Wise. S. P., and Desimone, R. (1988). Behavioral neurophysiology: Insights into seeing and grasping. Science, 242, 736–741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  191. Zeki, S. M. (1973). Colour coding in the rhesus monkey prestriate cortex. Brain Research, 53, 42 2427.Google Scholar
  192. Zeki, S. M. (1983). Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The responses of wavelength-selective and colour-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition. Neuroscience, 9, 767–781.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  193. Zillmer, E. A., Waechtler, C., Harris, B., Khan, F., and Fowler, P. C. (1992). The effects of unilateral and multifocal lesions on the WAIS-R: A factor analytic study of stroke patients. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 7. 29–40.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1995

Authors and Affiliations

  • Daniel X. Capruso
    • 1
  • Kerry deS. Hamsher
    • 2
  • Arthur L. Benton
    • 3
  1. 1.Department of NeurologyState University of New York at BuffaloBuffaloUSA
  2. 2.Department of NeurologyUniversity of Wisconsin Medical SchoolMilwaukeeUSA
  3. 3.Departments of Neurology and PsychologyUniversity of IowaIowa CityUSA

Personalised recommendations