Advertisement

Pigeon Homing: The Effect of Temporary Anosmia on Orientation Behavior

  • Wolfgang Wiltschko
  • Roswitha Wiltschko

Abstract

Carrier pigeons, Columba livia, re Leased at unfamiliar sites, do not depart randomly, but fly in directions that are normally not far from the homeward course. This indicates that the birds are aware in what direction their ‘home’ lies, and leads to the question: how do the birds know? Avian orientation has been the subject of intensive research during the last few decades, and some parts of this question can already be answered. Navigation is commonly described as a two-step process: In the first step, the map step, the birds determine their home direction as a compass course, and in the second step, they locate this course with the help of a compass and fly in the respective direction. The second step, the compass step, is rather well understood: Pigeons preferentially use a time-compensating sun compass which is, under overcast, backed up by a magnetic compass. The nature of the map step, however, is still largely unknown.

Keywords

Release Site Tonic Immobility Olfactory Information Pigeon Homing Home Direction 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Able, K. P. and Bingman, V. P., 1987, The development of orientation and navigation behavior in birds, Quart. Rev. Biol., 62: 1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Becker, J. and van Raden, H., 1986, Meteorologische Gesichtspunkte zur olfaktorischen Navigationshypothese, J. Ornithol., 127: 1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Keeton, W. T., 1973, Release-site bias as a possible guide to the “map” component in pigeon homing, J. Comp. Physiol., 86: 1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Keeton, W. T., 1980, Avian orientation and navigation: New developments in an old mystery, in: “Acta XVII Congr. Intern. Ornithol.”, Vol. I, R. Nöhring, ed., Deutsche Ornithologen-Gesellschaft, BerlinGoogle Scholar
  5. Kreithen, M. L., 1978, Sensory mechanisms for animal orientation — can any new ones be discovered?, in: “Animal Migration, Navigation and Homing”, K. Schmidt-Koenig and W. T. Keeton, eds., Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  6. Papi, F., 1982, Olfactory and homing in pigeons: Ten years of experiment, in: “Avian Navigation”, F. Papi and H. G. Wallraff, eds., Springer-Verlag, Berlin, Heidelberg, New YorkCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Papi, F., Keeton, W. T., Brown, A. I. and Benvenuti, S., 1978, Do American and Italian pigeons rely on different homing mechanisms?, J. Comp. Physiol., 128: 303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Schmidt-Koenig, K., 1987, Bird navigation: Has olfactory orientation solved the problem?, Quart. Rev. Biol., 62: 31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Walcott, C. and Lednor, A. J., 1983, Bird navigation, in: “Perspectives in Ornithology”, A. H. Brush and C. G. Clark, eds., Cambridge Univ. Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  10. Waldvogel, J. A., 1989, olfactory orientation by birds, in: “Current Ornithology”, Vol. 6, D. M. Power, ed., Plenum Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  11. Wallraff, H. G., 1980, Olfactory and homing in pigeons: Nerve-section experiments, critique, hypotheses, J. Comp. Physiol., 139: 209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Wallraff, H. G., 1986, Relevance of olfaction and atmospheric odours to pigeon homing, in: “Orientation on Space”, G. Beugnon, ed., Privat, I.E.C., ToulouseGoogle Scholar
  13. Wallraff, H. G., 1988, Olfactory deprivetion in pigeons: Examination of methods applied in homing experimerts, Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A, 89: 621PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Wallraff, H.G., 1989a, Simulated navigation based on unreliable sources of information (Models on pigeon homing, Part 1), J. Theor. Biol., 137: 1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Wallraff, H.G., 1989b, Simulated navigation based on assumed gradients of atmospheric trace gases (Models on pigeon homing, Part 2), J. Theor. Biol., 138: 511CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Wenzel, B. M. and Rausch, L. J., 1977, Does the olfactory system modulate affective behavior in the pigeon?, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 290: 314PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Wenzel, B. M., Albritton, P. F., Salzmann, A. and Oberjat, T. E., 1969, Behavioral changes on pigeons follcwing olfactory nerve section or bulb ablation, in: “Olfaction and laste”, Vol. 3, C. Pfaffman, ed., Rockefeller Univ. Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  18. Wiltschko, R. and Wiltschko, W., 1987, Pigeon homing: Olfactory experiments with inexperienced birds, Naturwissenschaften, 74: 94PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Wiltschko, R. and Wiltschko, W., 1989, Pigeon homing: Olfactory orientation — a paradox, Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 24: 163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Wiltschko, R., Schöps, M. and Kowalski, U., 1989, Pigon homing: wind exposition determines the importance of olfactory input, Naturwissenschaften 76: 229–231PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Wiltschko, W., Wiltschko, R., Foa, A. and Benvenuti, S., 1986, Orientation behaviour of pigeons deprived of olfactory information during the outward journey and at the release site, Monit. Zool. Ital. (N.S.), 20: 183Google Scholar
  22. Wiltschko, W., Wiltschko, R. and Walcott, C., 1987a, Pigoen homing: Different effects of olfactory deprivation in different countries, Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 21: 333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Wiltschko, W., Wilschtko, R. and Jahnel, M., 1987b, The orientation behavior of anosmic pigoens in Frankfurt a. M., Germany, Anim. Behay., 35: 1324CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Wolfgang Wiltschko
    • 1
  • Roswitha Wiltschko
    • 1
  1. 1.Fachbereich Biologie der UniversitätFrankfurt a.M.Germany

Personalised recommendations