The Social Ecology of Childhood

A Cross-Cultural View
  • Thomas S. Weisner
Chapter
Part of the Genesis of Behavior book series (GOBE, volume 4)

Abstract

Imagine that you could arrange for a satellite to circle the earth with extremely accurate high-resolution cameras and sound equipment on board, and that you could direct this satellite to hover over the culture areas of the world. Imagine that you could program your satellite to randomly sample communities, and within each community systematically select households with boys and girls of different ages, and that you could then monitor the members of those households throughout their day. Your cameras would record whom these family members are with; how far they stray from their home; what work they do; and with whom they do it. It would tell you the characteristics of the work and play groups, as well as the domestic group surrounding the child. It would tell you the kinds of physical stimulation the children are exposed to and how the children react and interact with their physical surroundings and social settings. Imagine that the microphones in your satellite could systematically sample what the children talk about; could study the content of their speech and with whom the children talk; and could observe the context of speech activities.

Keywords

Clay Migration Corn Income Cali 

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Barry, H., & Schlegel, A. (Eds.). Cross-cultural samples and codes. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, 1980.Google Scholar
  2. Bell, R., & Harper, L. Child effects on adults. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977.Google Scholar
  3. Boehm, C. A fresh outlook on cultural selection. American Anthropologist, 1982, 84, 105–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bronfenbrenner, U. The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1979.Google Scholar
  5. Cancian, F. Change and uncertainty in a peasant economy: The Maya corn farmers of Zincantan. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1972.Google Scholar
  6. Chibnick, M. The evolution of cultural rules. Journal of Anthropological Research, 1981, 37 (3), 256–268.Google Scholar
  7. Fjellman, S. Natural and unnatural decision making: A critique of decision theory. Ethos, 1976, 4 (1), 73–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Katz, M., & Konner, M. The role of the father: An anthropological perspective. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley, 1981.Google Scholar
  9. Leiderman, P. H., & Leiderman, G. F. Economic change and infant care in an East African agricultural community. In P. Leiderman, S. Tulkin, and A. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Culture and infancy. New York: Academic Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  10. LeVine, R. Child rearing as cultural adaptation. In P. H. Leiderman, S. Tulkin, and A. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Culture and infancy. New York: Academic Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  11. Lewis, M., & Ban, P. Variance and invariance in the mother—infant interaction: A cross-cultural study. In P. Leiderman, S. Tulkin, and A. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Culture and infancy. New York: Academic Press, 1977.Google Scholar
  12. Minturn, L., & Lambert, W. Mothers of six cultures: Antecedents of childrearing. New York: Wiley, 1964.Google Scholar
  13. Munroe, L., Munroe, R., & Whiting, B. Handbook of cross-cultural human development. New York: Garland, 1981.Google Scholar
  14. Sangree, W. H. Age, prayer and politics in Tiriki, Kenya. London: Oxford Press, 1966. Schwartz, T. The acquisition of culture. Ethos, 1981, 9 (1), 4–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Shweder, R. A., & D’Andrade, R. G. The systematic distortion hypothesis. In R. A. Shweder (Ed.), New directions for methodology of behavioral science: Fallible judgment in behavioral research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1980.Google Scholar
  16. Super, C., & Harkness, S. (Eds.). New directions for child development: Anthropological perspectives on child development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1980.Google Scholar
  17. Wagner, G. The Bantu of western Kenya. London: Oxford Press, 1949.Google Scholar
  18. Weisner, T. The structure of sociability: Urban migration and urban—rural ties in Kenya. Urban Anthropology, 1976, 5 (2), 199–223.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Weisner, T. Urban—rural differences in African children’s performance on cognitive and memory tasks. Ethos, 1976, 4 (2), 223–250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Weisner, T. Urban-rural differences in sociable and disruptive behaviors of Kenya children. Ethnology, 1979, 18 (2), 153–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Weisner, T. As we choose: Family life styles, social class, and compliance. In J. G. Kennedy and R. B. Edgerton (Eds.), Culture and ecology. Eclectic perspectives (Special publication No. 15 ). Washington, D. C.: American Anthropological Association, 1982.Google Scholar
  22. Weisner, T., & Abbott, S. Women, modernity, and stress: Three contrasting contexts for change in East Africa. Journal of Anthropological Research, 1977, 33 (4), 421–451.Google Scholar
  23. Weisner, T., & Gallimore, R. My brother’s keeper: Child and sibling caretaking. Current Anthropology, 1977, 18, 169–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Weisner, T., & Ross, M. The rural—urban migrant network in Kenya: Some general implications. American Ethnologist, 1977, 4 (2), 359–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Weisner, T., Bausano, M., & Kornfein, M. Putting family ideals into practice: Pro-natu- ralism in conventional and nonconventional California families. Ethos, 11 (4), 1983.Google Scholar
  26. Weisner, T., Gallimore, R., & Tharp, R. Concordance between ethnographer and folk perspectives: Observed performance and self-ascription of sibling caretaking roles. Human Organization, 41 (3), 1982, 237–244.Google Scholar
  27. Whiting, B. B. Transcultural code for social interaction. Unpublished manuscript, 1968.Google Scholar
  28. Whiting, B. B. Culture and social behavior: A model for the development of social behavior. Ethos, 1980, 8 (2), 95–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Whiting, B. B. The genesis of prosocial behavior. In D. Bridgeman (Ed.), The nature of prosocial development: Interdisciplinary theories of strategy. New York: Academic Press, 1983.Google Scholar
  30. Whiting, B. B., Edwards, C., et al. The company they keep: The genesis of gender role behavior, in preparation.Google Scholar
  31. Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. Children of six cultures: A psycho-cultural analysis. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1975.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1984

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas S. Weisner
    • 1
  1. 1.Departments of Psychiatry and AnthropologyUniversity of CaliforniaLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations