Advertisement

Auditory Pattern Perception in Infancy

  • Sandra E. Trehub
Part of the Advances in the Study of Communication and Affect book series (ASCA, volume 10)

Abstract

The study of auditory perception has been primarily the study of single sounds, and the study of pattern perception, that of visual patterns. Consider, instead, sequences of sounds as well as sets of single sounds, focusing not on acuity but on the perception of structure in these complex auditory patterns. For the present purposes, structure will refer to the relations between elements of a sound pattern or the relations between different members of a set of sounds. In this context, I will review the very limited research on auditory pattern perception in infancy and present some findings from our own laboratory.

Keywords

Acoustical Society Irrelevant Dimension Temporal Grouping Absolute Pitch Component Tone 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aaronson, D. Temporal factors in perception and short-term memory. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 67, 130–144.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aslin, R. N., Pisoni, D. B., & Jusczyk, P. W. Auditory development and speech perception in infancy. In P. Mussen & M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol 2): Perceptual Development. New York: Wiley, 1983.Google Scholar
  3. Attneave, F. Some informational aspects of visual perception. Psychological Review, 1954, 61, 183–193.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Attneave, F., & Olson, R. K. Pitch as a medium: A new approach to psychophysical scaling. American Journal of Psychology, 1971, 84, 147–166.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bartlett, J. C., & Dowling, W. J. Recognition of transposed melodies: A key-distance effect in developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1980, 6, 501–515.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bolton, T. L. Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 1894, 6, 145–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Boring, E. G. Sensation and perception in the history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1942.Google Scholar
  8. Bregman, A. S. Auditory streaming: Competition among alternative organizations. Perception and Psychophysics,1978, 23,391–398. (a)Google Scholar
  9. Bregman, A. S. Auditory streaming is cumulative. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,1978, 4,380–387. (b)Google Scholar
  10. Bregman, A. S. The formation of auditory streams. In J. Requin (Ed.), Attention and performance (Vol. VII). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1978. (c)Google Scholar
  11. Bregman, A. S. Asking the “what for” question in auditory perception. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.Google Scholar
  12. Bregman, A. S., & Campbell, J. Primary auditory stream segregation and perception of order in rapid sequences of tones. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1971, 89, 244–249.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bregman, A. S., & Dannenbring, G. L. The effect of continuity on auditory stream segregation. Perception and Psychophysics, 1973, 13, 308–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bregman, A. S., & Dannenbring, G. L. Auditory continuity and amplitude edges. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 1977, 31, 151–159.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bregman, A. S., & Rudnicky, A. I. Auditory segregation: Stream or streams ? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1975, 1, 263–267.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bregman, A. S., & Steiger, H. Auditory streaming and vertical localization: Interdependence of “what” and “where” in decisions in audition. Perception and Psychophysics,1980, 80,539546.Google Scholar
  17. Broadbent, D. E., & Ladefoged, P. Auditory perception of temporal order. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1959, 31, 1539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Chang, H. W., & Trehub, S. E. Auditory processing of relational information by young infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,1977, 24,324–331. (a)Google Scholar
  19. Chang, H. W., & Trehub, S. E. Infants’ perception of temporal grouping in auditory patterns. Child Development,1977, 48,1666–1670. (b)Google Scholar
  20. Dannenbring, G. L., & Bregman, A. S. Stream segregation and the illusion of overlap. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1976, 2, 544–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dannenbring, G. L., & Bregman, A. S. Streaming vs. fusion of sinusoidal components of complex tones. Perception and Psychophysics, 1978, 24, 369–376.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Demany, L. Auditory stream segregation in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 1982, 5, 261–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Demany, L., McKenzie, B., & Vurpillot, E. Rhythm perception in early infancy. Nature, 1977, 266, 718–719.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Deutsch, D. Music recognition. Psychological Review, 1969, 76, 300–307.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Deutsch, D. The organization of short-term memory for a single acoustic attribute. In D. Deutsch & J. A. Deutsch (Eds.), Short-term memory. New York: Academic Press, 1975.Google Scholar
  26. Divenyi, P. L., & Danner, W. F. Discrimination of time intervals marked by brief acoustic pulses of various intensities and spectra. Perception and Psychophysics, 1977, 21, 125–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Divenyi, P. L., & Sachs, R. M. Discrimination of time intervals bounded by tone bursts. Perception and Psychophysics, 1978, 24, 429–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Dowling, W. J. Scale and contour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychological Review, 1978, 85, 341–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Dowling, W. J. Melodic information processing and its development. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music. New York: Academic Press, 1982.Google Scholar
  30. Dowling, W. J., & Fujitani, D. A. Contour, interval, and pitch recognition in memory for melodies. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1971, 49, 524–531.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Eilers, R. E., Wilson, W. R., & Moore, J. M. Developmental changes in speech discrimination in infants. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1977, 20, 766–780.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Eimas, P. D. Auditory and phonetic coding of the cues for speech: Discrimination of the [r-1] distinction by young infants. Perception and Psychophysics, 1975, 18, 341–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Eimas, P. D., & Miller, J. Contextual effects in infant speech perception. Science, 1980, 209, 1140–1141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Endman, M. Perceptual constancy for nonspeech stimuli. Paper presented at the International Conference on Infant Studies, New York, April, 1984.Google Scholar
  35. Fitzgibbons, P. J., Pollatsek, A., & Thomas, I. B. Detection of temporal gaps within and between groups. Perception and Psychophysics, 1974, 16, 522–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Foulke, E. & Sticht, T. G. Review of research in the intelligibility and comprehension of accelerated speech. Psychological Bulletin, 1969, 72, 50–62.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Fraisse, P. Rhythm and tempo. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music. New York: Academic Press, 1982.Google Scholar
  38. Garner, W. R. Uncertainty and structure as psychological concepts. New York: Wiley, 1962.Google Scholar
  39. Gamer, W. R. The processing of information and structure. Potomac, Maryland: Erlbaum, 1974.Google Scholar
  40. Helmholtz, H. von. On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music. (A. J. Ellis, Ed. and trans.) New York: Dover, 1954. (Originally published in German, 1877.)Google Scholar
  41. Hochberg, J. Sensation and perception. In E. Hearst (Ed.), The first century of experimental psychology. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1979.Google Scholar
  42. Huttenlocher, J., & Burke, D. Why does memory span increase with age? Cognitive Psychology, 1976, 8, 1–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Jones, M. R. Auditory patterns: The perceiving organism. In E. C. Carterette & M. P. Friedman (Eds.), Handbook of perception (Vol. 8 ). New York: Academic Press, 1978.Google Scholar
  44. Kinney, D. K. & Kagan, J. Infant attention to auditory discrepancy. Child Development, 1976, 47, 155–164.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Koffka, K. The principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: Harcourt Brace, 1935.Google Scholar
  46. Kubovy, M. & Pomerantz, J. R. (Eds.). Perceptual organization. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.Google Scholar
  47. Kuhl, P. K. Speech perception in early infancy: Perceptual constancy for spectrally dissimilar vowel categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1979, 66, 1668–1679.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Kuhl, P. K. Perceptual constancy for speech-sound categories. In G. H. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanaugh, & C. A. Ferguson (Eds.), Child phonology (Vol. 2): Perception. New York: Academic Press, 1980.Google Scholar
  49. Kuhl, P. K. The perception of speech in early infancy: Four phenomena. In S. E. Gerber & G. T. Mencher (Eds.), The development of auditory behavior. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1983.Google Scholar
  50. Kuhl, P. K., & Miller, J. D. Discrimination of auditory target dimensions in the presence of variation in a second dimension by infants. Perception and Psychophysics, 1982, 31, 279–292.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Leavitt, L. A., Brown, J. W., Morse, P. A., & Graham, F. K. Cardiac orienting and auditory discrimination in 6-week infants. Developmental Psychology, 1976, 12, 514–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. MacDougall, R. The structure of simple rhythm forms. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements, 1903, 4, 309–416.Google Scholar
  53. McCall, R. B., & Melson, W. H. Amount of short-term familiarization and the response to auditory discrepancies. Child Development, 1970, 41, 861–869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Melson, W. H., & McCall, R. B. Attentional responses of five-month-old girls to discrepant auditory stimuli. Child Development, 1970, 41, 1159–1171.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Meumann, E. Untersuchungen zur Psychologie and Aesthetik des Rhythmus. Philosophische Studien, 1894, 10, 249–322, 393–430.Google Scholar
  56. Miner, J. B. Motor, visual, and applied rhythms: An experimental study and a revised explanation. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements,1903, 3,No. 4 (whole No. 21).Google Scholar
  57. Neisser, U. Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.Google Scholar
  58. Pick, A. D. Listening to melodies: Perceiving events. In A. D. Pick (Ed.), Perception and its development. New York: Wiley, 1979.Google Scholar
  59. Pomerantz, J. R. Perceptual organization in information processing. In M. Kubovy & J. R. Pomerantz (Eds.), Perceptual organization. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1981.Google Scholar
  60. Ryan, J. Temporal grouping, rehearsal and short-term memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 21, 148–155.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Seashore, C. E. Psychology of music. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1938.Google Scholar
  62. Terhardt, E., & Ward, W. D. Recognition of musical key: Exploratory study. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1982, 72, 26–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Thorpe, L. A., Trehub, S. E., & Morrongiello, B. A. Infants’ perception of melodies: Further observations on temporal grouping. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  64. Trehub, S. E. Infants’ sensitivity to vowel and tonal contrasts. Developmental Psychology, 1973, 9, 81–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Trehub, S. E. The discrimination of foreign speech contrasts by infants and adults. Child Development, 1976, 47, 466–472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Trehub, S. E., Bull, D., & Thorpe, L. A. Infants’ perception of melodies: The role of melodic contour. Child Development, 1984, 55, 821–830.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  67. Trehub, S. E., Thorpe, L. A., & Bull, D. Infants’ perception of melodies: The role of temporal grouping. Manuscript in preparation.Google Scholar
  68. Warren, R. M., Obusek, C. J., & Ackroff, J. M. Auditory induction: Perceptual synthesis of absent sounds. Science, 1972, 176, 1149–1151.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Warren, R. M., Obusek, C. J., Farmer, R. M., & Warren, R. P. Auditory sequence: Confusions of patterns other than speech or music. Science, 1969, 164, 586–587.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Williams, K. N., & Perrott, D. R. Temporal resolution of tonal impulses. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1972, 51, 644–647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Winckel, F. Music, sound, and sensation. New York: Dover, 1967.Google Scholar
  72. Winzenz, D., & Bower, G. H. Subject-imposed coding and memory for digit series. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1970, 83, 52–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Woodrow, H. A quantitative study of rhythm: The effect of variations in intensity, rate and duration. Archives of Psychology,1909, 18,No. 1.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1985

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sandra E. Trehub
    • 1
  1. 1.Centre for Research in Human DevelopmentErindale College, University of TorontoMississaugaCanada

Personalised recommendations