Advertisement

Assessing Functional Laterality

  • Jeffrey W. Gray
  • Raymond S. Dean
Part of the Critical Issues in Neuropsychology book series (CINP)

Abstract

A considerable amount of research evidence suggests that the cerebral hemispheres of the human brain serve independent cognitive functions. Neuropsychological assessments rely upon this concept in making inferences concerning the integrity of the brain. Because individual differences exist in the degree to which functions are lateralized to one hemisphere or the other, most neuropsychological approaches include some measure of laterality. Although this is a rather complex matter with adults, developmental factors complicate the procedure further with children.

Keywords

Left Hemisphere Hand Preference Lateral Preference Dichotic Listening Hemispheric Specialization 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 303–321.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Annett, M. (1976). A coordination of hand preference and skill replicated. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 587–592.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bakker, D. J. (1973). Hemispheric specialization and stages in the learning-to-read process. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 23, 15–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beaumont, J. G., & Rugg, M. D. (1978). Neuropsychological laterality of function and dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Dyslexia Review, 1, 18–21.Google Scholar
  5. Benton, A. L. (1975). Developmental dyslexia: Neurological aspects. In W. J. Friedlander (Ed.), Advances in neurology (Vol. 7, pp. 195–230). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  6. Berlin, C. I., & Cullen, J. K. (1977). Acoustic problems in dichotic listening tasks. In S. J. Segalowitz & F. A. Gruber (Eds.), Language development and neurological theory (pp. 75–88). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  7. Birkett, P. (1977). Measures of laterality and theories of hemispheric process. Neuropsychologia, 15, 693–696.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bogen, J. E., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1965). Cerebral commissurotoma in man: Minor hemisphere dominance for certain visuo-spatial functions. Journal of Neurosurgery, 23, 394–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Broadbent, P. E. (1954). The role of auditory localization in attention and memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47, 191–196.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Broca, P. (1960). Remarks on the seat of the faculty of articulate language, followed by an observation of aphemia. In G. Von Bonln (Ed). Some papers on the cerebral cortex. Springfield, IL: Thomas. (Original work published 1865).Google Scholar
  11. Bryden, M. P. (1982). Latemlity: Functional asymmetry in the intact brain. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  12. Critchley, M. (1970). The dyslexic child (2nd ed.). London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  13. Crovitz, H. F., & Zener, K. (1962). A group-test for assessing hand and eye dominance. American Journal of Psychology, 75, 271–276.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Davidson, R. J., & Schwartz, G. (1977). The influence of musical training on patterns of EEG asymmetry during musical and non-musical self-generalization tasks. Psychophysiology, 14, 58–63.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dean, R. S. (1978). Cerebral laterality and reading comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 16, 633–636.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Dean, R. S. (1979). Cerebral laterality and verbal-performance discrepancies in intelligence. Journal of School Psychology, 17, 145–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dean, R. S. (1981). Cerebral dominance and childhood learning disorders: Theoretical perspectives. School Psychology Review, 10, 373–378.Google Scholar
  18. Dean, R. S. (1982). Assessing patterns of lateral preference. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 4, 124–128.Google Scholar
  19. Dean, R. S. (1984). Functional lateralization of the brain. Journal of Special Education, 18, 240–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dean, R. S., & Hua, M. S. (1982). Laterality effects in cued auditory asymmetries. Neuropsychologia, 20, 685–690.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Dean, R. S., Schwartz, N. H., & Hua, M. (in press). Lateral preference patterns in schizophrenia and affective disorders, Journal of Abnormal Psychology.Google Scholar
  22. Dean, R. S., Schwartz, N. H., & Smith, L. S. (1981). Lateral reference patterns as a discriminator of learning disabilities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 227–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Doyle, J. C., Ornstein, R., & Galin, D. (1974). Lateral specialization of cognitive mode: II. EEG frequency analysis. Psychophysiology, 11, 567–578.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Dunlop, D. B., Dunlop, P., & Fenelon, B. (1973). Vision laterality analysis in children with reading disability: The results of new techniques of examination. Cortex, 9, 227–236.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Galin, D., Johnstone, J., & Herron, J. (1978). Effects of task difficulty on EEG measures of cerebral engagement. Neuropsychologia, 16, 461–472.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Galin, D., & Ornstein, R. (1972). Lateral specialization of cognitive mode: An EEG study. Psychophysiology, 9, 412–418.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gazzaniga, M. S. (1970). The bisected brain. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  28. Geffen, G., Traub, E., & Stierman, I. (1978). Language laterality assessed by unilateral ECT and dichotic monitoring. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 41, 354–360.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Geschwind, N. (1974). Selected papers on language and the brain. The Netherlands: D. Reidal.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gray, J. W., Dean, R. S., & Seretny, M. L. (1986). Lateral preference as a predictor of cognitive rigidity, Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 956–960.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Gruber, F. A., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1977). Some issues and methods in the neuropsychology of language. In S. J. Segalowitz & F. A. Gruber (Eds.), Language development and neurological theory (pp. 3–19). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  32. Hardyck, C., Petrinovich, L. F., & Goldman, R. D. (1977). Left-handedness and cognitive deficit. Cortex, 17, 266–279.Google Scholar
  33. Heron, W. (1957). Perception as a function of retinal locus and attention. American Journal of Psychology, 70, 38–48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hines, D., & Satz, P. (1977). Cross-model asymmetries in perception related to asymmetry in cerebral function. Neuropsychologia, 12, 239–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Humphrey, M. (1951). Handedness and cerebral dominance. Bachelor of Science thesis, Oxford University.Google Scholar
  36. Hynd, G. W., & Obrzut, J. E. (1977). Effects of grade level and sex on the magnitude of the dichotic ear advantage. Neuropsychologia, 15, 689–692.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hynd, G. W., Obrzut, J. E., Weed, W., & Hynd, C. R. (1979). Development of cerebral dominance: Dichotic lstening asymmetry in normal and learning-disabled children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 28, 445–454.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Jackson, J. H. (1874). On the duality of the brain. Medical Press, 1, 19. Reprinted in J. Taylor (Ed.), Selected writings of John Hughlings Jackson (Vol. 2). London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1932.Google Scholar
  39. Kershner, J. B. (1977). Cerebral dominance in disabled readers, good readers, and gifted children: Search for a valid model. Child Development, 48, 61–67.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kimura, D. (1961a). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 166–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Kimura, D. (1961b). Some effects of temporal lobe damage on auditory perception. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 156–165.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kimura, D. (1963). Right temporal lobe damage. Archives of Neurology, 8, 264–271.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Kimura, D., & Durnford, M. (1974). Normal studies on the function of the right hemisphere in vision. In S. J. Dimond & J. G. Beaumont (Eds.), Hemisphere functions in the human brain (pp. 159–175). London: Elek Scientific Books.Google Scholar
  44. Kinsbourne, M., & Hiscock, M. (1977). Does cerebral dominance develop? In S. J. Segalowitz & F. A. Gruber (Eds.), Language development and neurological theory (pp. 171–191). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  45. Lake, D., & Bryden, M. (1976). Handedness and sex differences in hemispheric asymmetry. Brain and Language. 3, 266–282.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Levy, J. (1969). Possible basis for the evaluation of lateral specialization of the human brain. Nature, 224, 614–615.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Luria, A. R. (1966). Human brain and Psychological processes. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  48. MacKinnon, G. E., Forde, J., & Piggens, D. J. (1969). Stabilized images, steadily fixated figures, and prolonged after images. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 23, 184–195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Majkowski, J., Bochenck, Z., Bochenck, W., Knapik-Fijalkowska, D., & Kopec, J. (1971). Latency of average evoked potentials to contralateral and ipsalateral auditory stimulation in normal subjects. Brain Research, 25, 416–419.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Marcel, T., Katz, L., & Smith, M. (1974). Laterality and reading proficiency. Neuropsychologia, 12, 131–139.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Marcel, T., & Rajan, P. (1975). Lateral specialization for recognition of words and faces in good and poor readers. Neuropsychologia, 13, 489–497.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. McKeever, W F., & Van Deventer, A. D. (1975). Dyslexic adolescents: Evidence of impaired visual and auditory language processing associated with normal lateralization and visual responsivity. Cortex, 11, 361–378.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences and psychological processes. British Medical Bulletin, 27, 272–277.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Milner, B. (1974). Hemispheric specialization: Scope and limits. In F. O. Schmitt & F. G. Warden (Eds.), The neurosciences: Third study programme (pp. 75–89). Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
  55. Milner, B., Branch, C., & Rasmussen, T. (1964). Observations on cerebral dominance. In A. V. S. DeRenck & M. O’Connor (Eds.), Disorders of language (CIBA Foundation Symposium) (pp. 200–241). London: Churchill.Google Scholar
  56. Molfese, D. L., Freeman, R. B., & Palermo, D. (1975). The ontogeny of brain lateralization for speech and nonspeech stimuli. Brain and Language, 2, 356–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Molfese, D. L., & Hess, T. M. (1978). Hemispheric specialization for VOT perception in the preschool child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26, 71–84.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1979a). Hemisphere and stimulus differences as reflected in the cortical responses of newborn infants to speech stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 15, 505–511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Molfese, D. L., & Molfese, V. J. (1979b). VOT distinctions in infants: Learned or innate. In H. A. Whitaker & H. Whitaker (Eds.), Studies in neurolinguistics (Vol. 4, pp. 21–35). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  60. Morgan, A., McDonald, P. J., & McDonald, H. (1971). Differences in bilateral alpha activity as a function of experimental tasks, with a note on lateral eye movements and hypnotizability. Neuropsychologia, 9, 459–469.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychoiogia, 9, 94–113.Google Scholar
  62. Orton, S. T. (1937). Specific reading disability—strephosymbolia. Journal of the American Medical Association, 90, 1095–1099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Peters, M., & Durding, B. (1978). Handedness as continuous variable. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 32, 257–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Pizzamiglio, L. (1976). Cognitive approach to hemispheric dominance. In R. M. Knights & D. Bakker (Eds.), The neuropsychology of learning disorders (pp. 265–272). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  65. Pritchard, R. M., Heron, W., & Hebb, D. O. (1960). Visual perception approached by the method of stabilized images. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 14, 66–77.Google Scholar
  66. Provins, K. A., & Cunliffe, P. (1972). Motor performance tests of handedness and motivation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35, 143–150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Raczkowski, D., Kalat, J. W., & Nebes, R. (1974). Reliability and validity of some handedness questionnaire items. Neuropsychologia, 12, 43–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Reitan, R. M. (1969). Manual for administration of neuropsychological test batteries for adults and children. Indianapolis: Author.Google Scholar
  69. Reynolds, C. R., Hartlage, L. C., & Haak, R. (1981). The relationship between lateral preference, as determined by neuropsychological test performance, and aptitude-achievement discrepancies. Clinical Neuropsychology, 3, 19–21.Google Scholar
  70. Sappington, J. T. (1980). Measures of lateral dominance: Interrelationships and temporal stability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 783–790.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Satz, P. (1972). Pathological left-handedness: An explanatory model. Cortex, 8, 121–135.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Satz, P. (1976). Cerebral dominance and reading disability: An old problem revisited. In R. M. Knights & D. Bakker (Eds.), The neuropsychology of learning disorders (pp. 273–294). Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
  73. Satz, P., Bakker, D. J., Teunissen, J., Goebel, R., & van der Vlugt, H. (1975). Developmental parameters of the ear asymmetry. A multivariate approach. Brain and Language, 2, 71–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Satz, P., Rardin, D., & Ross, J. (1971). An evaluation of a theory of specific developmental dyslexia. Child Development, 42, 2009–2021.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1975). A continuum of lateralization for speech perception. Brain and Language, 2, 212–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Shucard, D. W., Shucard, J. L., & Thomas, D. G. (1977). Auditory evoked potentials as probes of hemispheric differences in cognitive processing. Science, 197, 1295–1297.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Sparrow, S., & Satz, P. (1970). Dyslexia, laterality, and neuropsychological development. In D. J. Bakker & P. Satz (Eds.). Specific reading disability: Advances in theory and method (pp. 41–60). Rotterdam: Rotterdam University Press.Google Scholar
  78. Sperry, R. W., Gazzaniga, M. S., & Bogen, J. H. (1969). Interhemispheric relationships: The neocortical commissures: Syndromes of hemispheric disconnection. In P. Vinken & G. W. Bruyn (Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 4, pp. 273–290). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  79. Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Shankweiler, D. (1970). Hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48, 579–594.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Summers, R. K., & Taylor, M. L. (1972). Cerebral speech dominance in language-disordered and normal children. Cortex, 8, 224–232.Google Scholar
  81. Tjossem, T. D., Hansen, T. J., & Ripley, H. S. (1961). An investigation of reading difficulty in young children. Paper presented at the 117th annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago.Google Scholar
  82. Tomlinson-Keasey, C., & Kelly, R. R. (1979). Is hemispheric specialization important to scholastic achievement? Cortex, 15, 97–107.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Wada, J., Clark, R., & Hamm, A. (1975). Cerebral hemispheric asymmetry in humans. Archives of Neurology. 32, 239–246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Walker, H. A., & Birch, H. G. (1970). Lateral preference and right-left awareness in schizophrenic children. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 15, 341–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Witelson, S. F. (1976). Early hemispheric specialization and interhemisphere plasticity: An empirical and theoretical review. In S. Segalowitz & F. Gruber (Eds.), Language and development and neurologic theory (pp. 213–286). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  86. Witelson, S. F. (1977). Developmental dyslexia: Two right hemispheres and none left. Science, 195, 309–311.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Witelson, S. F., & Rabinovitch, M. S. (1972). Children’s recall strategies in dichotic listening. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 12, 106–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Woo, T. L., & Pearson, K. (1927). Dextrality and sinistrality. Biometrika, 19, 192–198.Google Scholar
  89. Zangwill, O. C. (1960). Cerebral dominance and its relationship to psychological function. London: Oliver & Boyd.Google Scholar
  90. Zangwill, O. C. (1962). Dyslexia in relation to cerebral dominance. In J. Money (Ed.), Reading disability (pp. 103–113). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins.Google Scholar
  91. Zurif, E. F., & Carson, G. (1970). Dyslexia in relation to cerebral dominance and temporal analysis. Neuropsychologia, 8, 351–361.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jeffrey W. Gray
    • 1
  • Raymond S. Dean
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Neuropsychology LaboratoryBall State UniversityMuncieUSA
  2. 2.University of Indiana School of MedicineMuncieUSA

Personalised recommendations