Advertisement

Human Experience and the Enigma of Culture: Toward an Enactive Account of Cultural Practice

  • Cor Baerveldt
  • Theo Verheggen
  • Paul Voestermans
Chapter

Summary

This paper deals with the way cultural psychology should deal with human experience. The common view about the relation between culture and experience holds that experience becomes “cultural” when people internalize or appropriate ready made cultural meanings. We contend that cultural forms themselves need to be dealt with in experiential terms. To this end we propose an “enactive” approach to cultural psychology. A central claim of enactivism is that experience is rooted within the organizational and operational autonomy of an acting system. Enactivism considers human experience to be constitutive for social and cultural phenomena. The main question of an enactive cultural psychology relates to the way human action becomes consensually coordinated. Both social psychologists who stress “sharedness” as the distinct mark of the social, and evolutionary psychologists who consider culture to derive from a uniform human mind, are criticized for overlooking the ongoing mutual tuning processes that give rise to socially and culturally patterned conduct.

Keywords

Anorexia Nervosa Human Experience Cultural Meaning Central Claim Symbolic Order 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Baerveldt, C. (1997). Cultural psychology as the study of meaning. Paper presented at the symposium “Unidades de Andlisis en la PsigologIa Cultural” at the XVII “Coloquio de Investigación”, 14–17 october, 1997, Universidad Nacional Autonoma de México, Escuela Nacional de Estudios Profesionales IztacalaGoogle Scholar
  2. Baerveldt, C (1998). Culture and the consensual coordination of actions. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Nijmegen.Google Scholar
  3. Baerveldt, C., and Verheggen, T. (1999a). Enactivism and the experiential reality of culture: Rethinking the epistemological basis of cultural psychology. Culture and Psychology, 5, 183206.Google Scholar
  4. Baerveldt, C., and Verheggen, T. (1999b). Towards a psychological study of culture: Epistemological considerations. In W. Maiers, B. Bayer, B. Duarte Esgalhado, R. Jorna, and E. Schraube (Eds), Challenges to theoretical psychology. North York,Canada: Captus.Google Scholar
  5. Baerveldt, C., and Voestermans, P. (1996). The body as a selfing device: The case of anorexia nervosa. Theory and Psychology, 6, 693–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York, NY: Ballantine.Google Scholar
  7. Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (1994). Beyond intuition and instinct blindness: Toward an evolutionary rigorous cognitive science. Cognition, 50, 41–77.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Edelman, G. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  10. Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M•(1998). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  11. Maturana, H. (1978). Biology of language: The epistemology of reality. In G. A. Miller and E. Lenneberg (Eds.), Psychology and biology of language and thought: Essays in honour of Eric Lenneberg (pp. 27–63 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  12. Maturana, H. (1980). Man and society. In F. Benseler, P.M. Hejl, and W.K. Köck (Eds), Autopoiesis, communication and society: The theory of autopoietic systems in the social sciences (pp. 11–32 ). Frankfurt: Campus Verlag.Google Scholar
  13. Maturana, H. (1988). Reality: The search for objectivity or the quest for a compelling argument. The Irish Journal of Psychology, 9, 25–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Maturana, H., and Varela, F. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living. Dordrecht: Reidel Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Shweder, R.A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Spencer Brown, G. (1969). Laws ofform. London: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
  17. Varela, F. (1979). Principles of biological autonomy. New York: Elsevier North Holland.Google Scholar
  18. Varela, F.J., Thompson, F., and Rosch, E. (1991). The Embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  19. Voestermans, P. (1991). Alterity/Identity: A deficient image of culture. In J. Leerssen and R. Corbey (Eds), Alterity, identity, image: Selves and others in society and scholarship (pp. 219–250 ). Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar
  20. Von Glasersfeld, E. (1991). Knowing without metaphysics: Aspects of the radical constructivist position. In F. Steier (Ed.), Research and reflexivity. London: Sage.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2001

Authors and Affiliations

  • Cor Baerveldt
    • 1
  • Theo Verheggen
    • 1
  • Paul Voestermans
    • 1
  1. 1.University of NijmegenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations