Skip to main content

Dimensional Color: Theoretical Background

  • Chapter
  • 115 Accesses

Part of the book series: Design Science Collection ((DSC))

Abstract

The dimensions of color can be regarded as a function of vision itself. Fascinating as they are, theories of perception or an understanding of the physiology of the eye, are issues apart from those of visual invention. Central to this study is that the perception of color is integral with the perception of form. Therefore a brief discussion of some theories may illuminate the experiments that follow.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   64.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. F. Allport, Theories of Perception and Concept of Structure (New York: Wiley, 1955).

    Google Scholar 

  2. F. Allport, Theories of Perception and Concept of Structure (New York: Wiley, 1955)**Ibid., p. 120, quotes Koffka.

    Google Scholar 

  3. F. Allport, Theories of Perception and Concept of Structure (New York: Wiley, 1955)**Ibid., pp. 598–605.

    Google Scholar 

  4. J.J. Gibson, Perception of the Visual World (New York): Houghton-Mifflin, 1950). Making the distinction between the visual world, which is external to the human observer, and the visual field, within which these clues interrelate, Gibson redefines the abstract conception of the picture plane of perspectival space.

    Google Scholar 

  5. J.J. Gibson, The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems (Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1966). Visual relationship, as expressed by gradients (consistent incremental changes such as size, texture, density) give rise to parallel processing in the cortex.

    Google Scholar 

  6. D. Katz, The World of Colour (London: Kegan Paul, 1935). Katz links the loss of surface with the imperceptibility of microstructure or detail. The conversion of a surface to film color results in this condition.

    Google Scholar 

  7. The Liebmann effect is illustrated in a paper study by Josef Albers, using two hues of equal light intensity. When juxtaposed on a plane, the boundary between the two colors is imperceptible.

    Google Scholar 

  8. H. Helson and E. Fehrer, “The Role of Form in Perception, “American Journal of Psychology 44 (1932): 79–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Defined by Edgar Rubin. The first systematic study of figure/ground—one of his rules says that the enclosed surface tends to be perceived as figure, while the enclosing one will be ground.

    Google Scholar 

  10. J.J. Gibson, Perception of the Visual World p. 11.

    Google Scholar 

  11. R.W. Burnham, “The Dependence of Color upon Area,” The American Psychologist 4 (1949): 230–231.

    Google Scholar 

  12. F. Birren, Color Psychology and Color Therapy (New York: McGraw-Hill, 1950), p. 146.

    Google Scholar 

  13. D. Katz, The World of Colour pp. 7–28.

    Google Scholar 

  14. M. Martin, “Film, Surface and Bulky Colors and Their Intermediaries,” American Journal of Psychology 33 (1922): 451–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. D. Katz, The World of Colour.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Goethe, Theory of Colors\ p. 60.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Notes taken by the author in Albers’ color course at Yale University in 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  18. J. Albers, The Interaction of Color (New Haven, Conn: Yale University, 1963).

    Google Scholar 

  19. Chevreul, The Principles of Harmony and Contrast of Colors. (New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1967).

    Google Scholar 

  20. C. Ladd-Franklin, Color and Color Theories (New York: Harcourt-Brace, 1929), p. 49: “Hering made it clear that simultaneously induced color was a retinal phenomenon. His theory posits the retinal surface as containing receptor cells sensitive to complementary pairs of colors. The influence of a color on the receptor activates immediately the opposite process. His studies of the after-image and simultaneous light and color induction, place the contrast phenomenon as physiological in character.”

    Google Scholar 

  21. Ewald Hering, Outline of a Theory of the Tight Sense translated by Leo Hurvich and Dorothea Jameson (Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University, 1964).

    Google Scholar 

  22. R. Henneman, “A Photometric Study of the Perception of Object Color,” Archives of Psycholog)!, Ph.D. Columbia University, New York, 1935.

    Google Scholar 

  23. “ Sunday Afternoon on the Island of la Grande Jatte.” Collection, The Art Institute of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Leo Hurvich and Dorothea Jameson, “From Contrast to Assimilation; in Art and in the Eye,” Pergamon, Leonardo 8 (1975): 125–131. The effect of additive mixtures by their assimilation in the eye is described with respect to its occurrence in the work of the pointillist painters, and scientifically, in the optical response.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. E. Hering, Theory of the Light Sense.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Hermann von Helmholtz, Treatise on Psychological Optics (New York: Dover, 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Edwin Land, “The Retinex,” The American Scientist 52, 10.2 (1964).

    Google Scholar 

  28. C. Ladd-Franklin, Color and Color Theories.

    Google Scholar 

  29. E.T. Hall, The Hidden Dimension (New York: Doubleday, 1966).

    Google Scholar 

  30. A.H. Munsell, A Color Notation, 12th ed. (Baltimore: Munsell Color Company, 1975).

    Google Scholar 

  31. W. Ostwald, The Color Primer (New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1969).

    Google Scholar 

  32. P. Bouma, The Physical Aspects of Color (The Netherlands: Eindhoven Phillips Gloeilampenfabrieken, 1947).

    Google Scholar 

  33. The Weber-Fechner law—The arithmetic appearance of light gradation is due to a geometric progression of the stimulus.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Albers, Interaction of Color.

    Google Scholar 

  35. The term aerial perspective denotes the optical change in surfaces seen at great distances. Their distinctiveness as structures diminishes and at the same time all colors are influenced by the appearance of blue. The term is found in Leonardo’s notebooks, and in Alberti’s Treatise on Painting.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Koschmieder defined the theory of visual range in 1924, from R.A.R. Tricker, Introduction to Meteorological Optics (New York: American Elsevier, 1970).

    Google Scholar 

  37. Fechner’s psychophysical law. As the distance from the stimulus increases relative to the eye, the contrast between adjacent areas decreases in intensity.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Swirnoff, L. (1989). Dimensional Color: Theoretical Background. In: Dimensional Color. Design Science Collection. Birkhäuser, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-2073-0_3

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-2073-0_3

  • Publisher Name: Birkhäuser, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-2075-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-2073-0

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics