Responses to Consistent and Discrepant Social Communications

  • Fred R. Volkmar
  • Alberta E. Siegel


A naive assertion about human communication is that it consists of words. A more sophisticated statement recognizes that extralexical aspects of communication, including gestures, posture, facial expression, tone of voice, dress, and timing of speech, also convey meaning. In human social interaction information, typically, is conveyed redundantly so that a speaker’s facial expressions, gestures, tone of voice, postures, and words are mutually consistent, augmenting and reinforcing each other. For purposes of analysis the various communication components may be grouped into “channels” which are defined as sets of communication components which convey information and which are emitted and responded to in a systematic manner (Wiener & Mehrabian, 1968). Although the delineation of any set of component behaviors as belonging to the same communication channel is somewhat arbitrary, it can be justified pragmatically and empirically (Wiener, Devoe, Rubinow, & Geller, 1972).


Facial Expression Nonverbal Communication Visual Channel Parental Communication Objective Meaning 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ.: Erlbaum, 1978.Google Scholar
  2. Anderson, D. R., Lorch, E. P., Field, D. E., & Sanders, J. The effects of TV program comprehensibility on preschool children’s visual attention to television. Child Development, 1981, 52, 151–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bateson, G., Jackson, D. D., Haley, J., & Weakland, J. Toward a theory of schizophrenia. Behavioral Science, 1956, 1, 241–264.Google Scholar
  4. Beakel, N. G., & Mehrabian, A. Inconsistent communications and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1969, 74, 126–130.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Birch, H. G. & Lefford, A. Intersensory development in children. Mongraphs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1963, 28 (2, Whole No. 89).Google Scholar
  6. Bronson, G. W. Infants’ reactions to unfamiliar persons and novel objects. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1972, 37 (3, Whole No. 148).Google Scholar
  7. Brooks, R., Brandt, L., & Wiener, M. Differential response to two communication channels: Socioeconomic class differences in response to verbal reinforcers communicated with and without tonal inflection. Child Development, 1969, 40, 453–470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Buck, R. Nonverbal communication of affect in children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1975 31, 644–653.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buck, R. Nonverbal communication of affect in preschool children: Relationships with personality and skin conductance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1977, 35, 225–236.Google Scholar
  10. Bugental, D. C. Interpretations of naturally occurring discrepancies between words and intonation, modes of inconsistency resolution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1974, 30, 125–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bugental, E. E., Kaswan, J. W., & Love, L. R. Perception of contradictory meanings conveyed by verbal and nonverbal channels. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1970, 16, 647–655. (a)PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bugental, D. E., Kaswan, J. W., Love, L. R., & Fox, M. N. Child versus adult perception of evaluative messages in verbal, vocal, and visual channels. Developmental Psychology, 1970, 2, 367–375. (b)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bugental, D. E. Love, L. R. Kaswan, J. W., & April, C. Verbal-nonverbal conflict in parental messages to normal and disturbed children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1971, 77, 6–10.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bugental, D. E., Love, L. R., & Gianetto, R. M Perfidious feminine faces. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1971, 17, 314–318.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bugental, D. B., & Love, L. Nonassertive expression of parental approval and disapproval and its relationship to child disturbance. Child Development, 1975, 46, 747–752.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Collins, W. A. Cognitive processing aspects of TV viewing. In D. Pearl (Ed.), Television and Behavior: Ten years of Scientific Progress, NIMH, Washington, DC, 1982.Google Scholar
  17. DePaulo, B. M. & Rosenthal, R. Age changes in nonverbal decoding as a function of increasing amounts of information. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1978, 26, 280–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Harper, R. G., Wiens, A. W., & Matarazzo, J. D. Nonverbal communication: The state of the art. New York: Wiley, 1978.Google Scholar
  19. Hinde, R. A. Biological bases of human social behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1974.Google Scholar
  20. Jancovic, M., Devoe, S., & Wiener, M. Age-related changes in hand and arm movements as nonverbal communication: Some conceptualizations and an empirical exploration. Child Development, 1975, 46, 922–928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Levin, S. R. & Anderson, D. R. The development of attention. Journal of Communication, 1976, 26, 126–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Levitt, E. A. The relationship between abilities to express emotional meanings vocally and facially. In J. R. Davitz (Ed.), The communication of emotional meaning. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1964.Google Scholar
  23. Mayo, C. & LaFrance, M. On the acquisition of nonverbal communication: A review. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 1978, 24, 213–228.Google Scholar
  24. Mehrabian, A. & Ferris, S. R. Inference of attitudes from nonverbal communication. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1967, 31, 248–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Mehrabian, A. & Wiener, M. Decoding of inconsistent communications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1967, 6, 109–114.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Peterson, C. L., Danner, F. W., & Flavell, J. H. Developmental changes in children’s response to three indications of communicative failure. Child Development, 1972, 43, 1463–1468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Piaget, J. The origins of intelligence in children. New York: Norton, 1963.Google Scholar
  28. Prentice, N. M. & Fathman, R. E. Joking riddles: A developmental index of children’s humor. Developmental Psychology, 1975, 11, 210–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Rheingold, H. L. The effect of a strange environment on the behavior of infants. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of infant behavior (Vol. 4 ). London: Metheun, 1969.Google Scholar
  30. Rice, M. L., Houston, A. C., & Wright, J. C. The forms and codes of television: Effects on children’s attention, comprehension and social behavior. In D. Pearl, (Ed.), Television and behavior: Ten years of scientific progress, NIMH, Washington, DC, 1982.Google Scholar
  31. Schuam, A. I. The double-bind hypothesis a decade later. Psychological Bulletin, 1967, 68, 409–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Solomon, D., & Ali, F. A. Age trends in the perception of verbal reinforcers. Developmental Psychology, 1972, 7, 288–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Solomon, D. & Yaeger, J. Determinants of boys’ perceptions of verbal reinforcers. Developmental Psychology, 1969, 1, 637–645. (a)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Solomon, D. & Yaeger, J. Effects of content and intonation on perceptions of verbal reinforcers. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1969, 28, 319–327. (b)PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Volkmar, F. R. & Cohen, D. J. A hierarchical analysis of patterns of noncompliance in autistic and behavior disturbed children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1982, 12, 35–42.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Volkmar, F. R., Hoder, E. L., & Siegel, A. E. Discrepant social communications. Developmental Psychology, 1980, 16, 495–505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Volkmar, F. R. & Siegel, A. E. Young children’s responses to discrepant social communications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1979, 20, 139–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wiener, M. & Mehrabian, A. Language within language. New York: Appleton, Century, Croft, 1968.Google Scholar
  39. Weiner, M., Devoe, S., Rubinow, S., & Geller, J. Nonverbal behavior and nonverbal communication. Psychological Review, 1972, 79, 185–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Zigler, E. & Kanzer, P. The effectiveness of two classes of reinforcers on the performance of middle-class and lower-class children. Journal of Personality, 1962, 30, 157–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Fred R. Volkmar
  • Alberta E. Siegel

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations