Advertisement

Non-Verbal Communication Devices: Their Relevance, Their Use and the Mental Processes Involved

  • F. Lowenthal

Abstract

Non-verbal communication devices have been defined in previous papers (Lowenthal, 1978a, 1982). We want to show here, by means of examples, in what ways they are similar to and how they differ from any known verbal language; what makes them useful either for introducing new concepts in a teaching situation, or for creating a situation where the subject’s behaviour can easily be observed and analyzed. Finally we will describe what mental processes are involved, according to us, when children are confronted with NVCDs. This can be illustrated by descriptions of experiments carried out with children (Lowenthal, 1979, 1984a). Two of these experiments are presented in this book (the first one concerns the use of NVCDs in a teaching situation and the second one concerns the use of an NVCD used to observe the behaviour of children solving a logical problem).

Keywords

Logical Structure Finite Automaton Communication Device Verbal Language Technical Constraint 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bruner, J.S., 1966a, Toward a theory of instruction, Harvard University Press, Cambridge (U.S.A.).Google Scholar
  2. Bruner, J.S., et al., 1966b, Studies in cognitive growth, John Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  3. Cohors-Fresenborg, E., 1978, Learning problem solving by developing automata networks, Revue de Phonétique Appliquée 46 /47, 93–99.Google Scholar
  4. Cohors-Fresenborg, E., and Griep, M., 1982, Registermachinen und Funktionen, Osnabrücker Schriften zur Mathematik Universität Osnabrück.Google Scholar
  5. Cordier, J., Lowenthal, F., and Heraux, C., 1975, Enseignement de la mathématique et exercices de verbalisation chez les enfants caractériels, Enfance, 1, 111–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Dienes, Z.P., and Golding, E.W., 1970, Les premiers pas en mathématique: logique et jeux logiques, OCDL, Paris.Google Scholar
  7. Freudenthal, H., 1973, Mathematics as an educational task, Reidel, Dordrecht.Google Scholar
  8. Harmegnies, B., and Lowenthal, F., 1984, Dispositifs de communication non-verbale et ordinateurs, Humankybernetik, 25 (3), 115–124.Google Scholar
  9. Laborde, C., and Guillerault, M., 1984, Learning to formulate in mathematics: imitation or creation?, in Language and Mathematics–interactions between their developments, ed. Lowenthal F., Laboratoire N.V.C.D., University of Mons, 47–60.Google Scholar
  10. Lewis Carroll, 1966, Logique sans peine, Herman, Paris.Google Scholar
  11. Lowenthal, F., 1978a, Logic of natural language and games at primary school, Revue de Phonétique Appliquée 46 /47, 133–140.Google Scholar
  12. Lowenthal, F., 1978b, Logic and language in game setting–for children ages 8 to 10, in: Proceedings of the second international conference for the psychology of mathematics education, eds. Cohors-Fresenborg, E. and Wachsmuth, I., Osnabrücker Schriften zur Mathematik, Reihe D., Mathematisch-didaktische Manuskripte Band 1, 214–225.Google Scholar
  13. Lowenthal, F., 1979, Hypothetico-deductive reasoning at the age of 8–clinical observations, in: Theory of Knowledge and Science Policy, eds. Callebaut, W., De Mey, M., Pinxten, R and Vandamme, F., Communicatie en Cognitie, Gent, 149–159.Google Scholar
  14. Lowenthal, F., 1980, Games, logic and cognitive development–a longitudinal study of classroom situations, Communication and Cognition 13, 1, 43–63.Google Scholar
  15. Lowenthal, F., 1982, Example of auxiliary formalisms used to help the development of children’s logical thinking, in Language and Language Acquisition, eds. Lowenthal, F., Vandamme, F. and Cordier, J., Plenum Press, New York, 113–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lowenthal, F., 1984a, Production langagière d’enfants manipulant un dispositif non-verbal de communication, Revue de Phonétique Appliquée 69, 11–46Google Scholar
  17. Lowenthal, F., 1984b, Development of communicative abilities and the acquisition of mathematical concepts in children, in Language and Mathematics Interactions between their developments, ed. Lowenthal, F., Laboratoire N.V.C.D., University of Mons, 19–60.Google Scholar
  18. Lowenthal, F., 1985, Non-verbal communication devices in language acquisition, Revue de Phonétique Appliquée 73/74/75.Google Scholar
  19. Lowenthal, F., and Severs, R., 1979, Langage, jeu et activité mathématique–un essai â l’école primaire, Educational Studies in Mathematitics, 10, 245–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Lowenthal, F. and Marcq, J., 1981, Logic Auxiliary Formalism and Geometry by Telephone Call, in: Proceedings of the 5thGoogle Scholar
  21. conference of the international group for psychology of mathematics education, Vol. I, eds. Comiti C., and Vergnaud, G., Laboratoire IMAG, Grenoble, 265–270.Google Scholar
  22. Lowenthal, F. and Marcq, J., 1982, How do Children discover strategies (at the age of 7)?, in: Proceedings of the 6th conferenceGoogle Scholar
  23. Psychology of Mathematics Education, Ed. Vermandel, A., Universitaire Instelling Antwerpen, Antwerpen, 287–292.Google Scholar
  24. Papert, S., 1980, Mindstorms, Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
  25. Papy, F., 1977, Math Playtherapy I, Cemrel, St. Louis.Google Scholar
  26. Papy, F., Heidema, C. and Schweitzer, J., 1969, CSMP MathematicsGoogle Scholar
  27. for the upper grades, Part II, Teacher’s guide, Cemrel, St. Louis.Google Scholar
  28. Papy, G., 1969, Minicomputer, Educational Studies in Mathematics, 33 (2), 3–45.Google Scholar
  29. Piaget, J., 1936, La naissance de l’intelligence chez l’enfant, Delachaux and Niestle, Neuchatel.Google Scholar
  30. Piaget, J., 1947, La représentation du monde chez l’enfant, Presses Universitaires de France, Paris.Google Scholar
  31. Renard, R., 1985, Structuro-globalism and autonomy, Revue de Phonétique Appliquée, 73/74/75.Google Scholar
  32. Saerens, J., 1985, Semantic components and syntactic sequences introduced by means of mutlicoloured plastic pegs, Communication and Cognition Vol. 18 (1), 24–28.Google Scholar
  33. Siegel, L., 1978, The Relationship of Language and Thought in the Pre-operational child: A Reconsideration of Non-Verbal Alternatives to Piagetian Tasks, in Alternatives to Piaget–Critical Essays on the Theory, eds., Siegel, L. and Brainerd, C., Academic Press, New York, 43–67.Google Scholar
  34. Siegel, L., 1982, The discrepancy between Cognitive and Linguistic Abilities in the Young Child, in Language and Language Acquisition, eds. Lowenthal, F., Vandamme, F. and Cordier J., Plenum Press, New York, 31–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Weinzweig, A.I., 1980, Concepts, Language and Context, Revue de Phonétique Appliquée 55 /56; 267–280.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • F. Lowenthal
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratoire NVCDUniversity of MonsBelgium

Personalised recommendations