Individual Differences Associated with Lucid Dreaming

  • Thomas J. Snyder
  • Jayne Gackenbach

Abstract

Lucid dreaming has been said to be within the capability of all individuals (LaBerge, 1985). Based on analyses of the incidence of this dream experience among university students and among persons with an expressed interest in dreaming, a majority have reported experiencing at least one lucid dream during their lifetime, and about 20% have reported experiencing lucid dreams with relative frequency. Our goal in this chapter is to describe and to integrate what has been learned through research about individuals who experience lucid dreams. To this end we will present data derived from the study of four separable but not unrelated functional domains for which subject differences associated with lucid dreaming, or lucidity, have been found. These functional domains are (1) oculomotor/equilibratory; (2) visual/imaginal; (3) intellectual/creative, and (4) personal/interpersonal. The extent of individual differences in lucid dreaming and the methods by which these differences have been investigated will also be discussed. Because methodology is an integral part of research into individual differences, methodological considerations will first be presented.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bakan, P. (1969). Hypnotizability, laterality of eye movements and functional brain asymmetry. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 41, 85–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bakan, P. (1971). The eyes have it. Psychology Today, pp. 64–68.Google Scholar
  3. Bakan, P., & Shotland, R. (1969). Lateral eye movement, reading speed, and visual attention. Psychonomic Science, 15, 93–94.Google Scholar
  4. Barker, R. G. (1931). The stepping-stone maze: A directly visible space problem apparatus. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 280–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Belicki, D. A., Hunt, & Belicki, K. (1978). An exploratory study comparing self-reported lucid and non-lucid dreamers. Sleep Research, 7, 166.Google Scholar
  6. Bem, S. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Blackmore, S. J. (1982). More sex differences in lucid dreaming frequency. Lucidity Letter, 1(2), 5. Blackmore, S. J. (1983). A survey of lucid dreams, OBE’s, and related experiences. Lucidity Letter, 2(3), 1.Google Scholar
  8. Blackmore, S. J. (1984). A postal survey of OBE’s and other experiences. Journal of the Society for Psychical Research, 52, 225–244.Google Scholar
  9. Bloomberg, M. (1971). Creativity as related to field independence and mobility. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 118, 3–12.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bloomberg, M. (1976). An inquiry into the relationship between field independence-dependence and creativity. The Journal of Psychology, 67, 127–140.Google Scholar
  11. Buss, D. M., & Craik, K. H. (1983). The act frequency approach to personality. Psychological Review, 90(2), 105–126.Google Scholar
  12. Cartwright, D. (1971). Risk taking by individuals and groups: An assessment of research employing choice dilemmas. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20(3), 361–378.Google Scholar
  13. Cartwright, R. D. (1978). A primer on sleep and dreaming. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  14. Cattell, R. (1969). 16PF. Champaign, IL: The Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. Chatterjea, R. G., Bhaskar, P. (1980). Field dependence, sex, introversion, extroversion, and social desirability. Journal of Psychological Researches, 24, 115–120.Google Scholar
  15. Constantinople, A. (1973). Masculinity-femininity: An exception to a famous dictum? Psychological Bulletin, 80, 389–407.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Cook, A. M., & Irwin, H. J. (1983). Visuospatial skills and the out-of-body experience. Journal of Parapsychology, 47, 23–35.Google Scholar
  17. Corcoran, D. W. J. (1981). Introversion-extroversion, stress, and arousal. In D. E. Broadbent R. Lynn (Eds.), Dimensions of personality: Papers in honour of H. J. Eysenck. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  18. Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964). The approval motive. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  19. Dane, Joe. (1984). A comparison of waking instructions and post hypnotic suggestion for lucid dream induction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Georgia State University, Atlanta.Google Scholar
  20. DeWitt, G. W., Averill, J. R. (1976). Lateral eye movements, hypnotic susceptibility and field independence-dependence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 1179–1184.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Doneshka, P., Kehiyov, A. (1978). Some peculiarities of the dreams of patients with vestibular diseases. Acta Medica (Irregular), 32 (1), 45–50.Google Scholar
  22. Ehrlichman, H., & Weinberger, A. (1978). Lateral eye movements and hemispheric asymmetry: A critical view. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1080–1101.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Eisinger, K., & Schilder, P. (1929). Dreams and labyrinth lesions. Psychiatria et Neurologia, 73, 314–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Eysenck, H. J. (1982). Personality, genetics, and behavior: Selected papers. New York: Praeger Publishers.Google Scholar
  25. Fenigstein, A., & Scheier, M. F., Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43 (4), 522–527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fenwick, P. B. C., & Schatzman, M., Worsley, A., Adams, J., Stone S., Baker, A. (1984). Lucid dreaming: Correspondence between dreamed and actual events in one subject during REM sleep. Biological Psychology, 18, 243–252.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Forisha, B. L. (1978). Mental imagery and creativity: Review and speculations. Journal of Mental Imagery, 2, 209–238.Google Scholar
  28. Gackenbach, J. I. (1978). A personality and cognitive style analysis of lucid dreaming. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Virginia Commonwealth University.Google Scholar
  29. Gackenbach, J. I. (1980). [Fall mass testing]. Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
  30. Gackenbach, J. I. (1983). [Fall mass testing]. Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
  31. Gackenbach, J. I. (1984a). [Spring mass testing]. Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
  32. Gackenbach, J. I. (1984b). [Summer mass testing]. Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
  33. Gackenbach, J. I. ( 1985a, June). Eye movement direction and the lucid dreaming ability. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for the Study of Dreams, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA.Google Scholar
  34. Gackenbach, J. I. (1985b). Sex differences in lucid dreaming frequency: A second look. Lucidity Letter, 4(1), 11.Google Scholar
  35. Gackenbach, J. I. (1986). Personality differences between individuals varying in lucid dreaming frequency. Manscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  36. Gackenbach, J. I., & Hammons, S. (1983). Lucid dreaming ability and verbal creativity. Dream-works, 3(3), 219–223.Google Scholar
  37. Gackenbach, J. I., Schillig, B. (1983). Lucid dreams: The content of conscious awareness of dreaming during the dream. Journal of Mental Imagery, 7 (2), 1–14.Google Scholar
  38. Gackenbach, J. I., & Snyder, T. J., Esbeck, S. (1981). [Visual maze task]. Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
  39. Gackenbach, J. I., Snyder, T. J., McKelvey, K., McWilliams, C., George, E., Rodenelli, B. (1981). Lucid dreaming: Individual differences in perception. Sleep Research, 10, 146.Google Scholar
  40. Gackenbach, J. I., & Snyder, T. J., Vognsen, E. (1981). [Tactile maze task]. Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
  41. Gackenbach, J. I., & Sachau, D., Rokes, L. (1982). Vestibular sensitivity and dynamic and static motor balance as a function of sex and lucid dreaming. Sleep Research, 11, 104.Google Scholar
  42. Gackenbach, J. I., Curren, R., LaBerge, S., Davidson, D., & Maxwell, P. ( 1983, June). Intelligence, creativity, and personality differences between individuals who vary in self-reported lucid dreaming frequency. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Association for the Study of Mental Imagery, Vancouver.Google Scholar
  43. Gackenbach, J. I., Curren, R., & Cutler, G. ( 1983, June). Presleep determinants and post-sleep results of lucid versus vivid dreams. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Association for the Study of Mental Imagery, Vancouver.Google Scholar
  44. Gackenbach, J. I., Prill, S. & Westrom, P. (1983). The relationship of the lucid dreaming ability to mental imagery experiences and skills. Lucidity Letter, 2 (4), 4–6.Google Scholar
  45. Gackenbach, J. I., Heilman, N., Boyt, S., LaBerge, S. (1985). The relationship between field independence and lucid dreaming ability. Journal of Mental Imagery, 9 (1), 9–20.Google Scholar
  46. Gackenbach, J. I., & Sachau, D., Rokes, L., Snyder, T. J. (1986). Lucid dreaming ability as a function of gross motor balance. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  47. Gackenbach, J. I., Snyder, T. J., Rokes, L., Sachau, D., (1986). Lucid dreaming frequency in relationship to vestibular sensitivity as measured by caloric stimulation. In R. Haskel (Ed.) Cognition and Dream Research: The Journal of Mind and Behavior (special issue), 7(2/3), 277–298.Google Scholar
  48. Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligence. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  49. Gilgen, A. R., & Cho, J. H. (1979). Questionnaire to measure Eastern and Western thought. Psychological Reports, 44, 835–841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Golden, C. J., Hemmeke, R. A., Purisch, A. D. (1979). The standardization Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery: A manual for clinical and experimental use. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  51. Goldenberg, Y. M. (1979). The relationship of sex, sex-role orientation, and self-esteem to attitudes toward risk-taking. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Adelphi University.Google Scholar
  52. Gough, H. G. (1968). An interpreter’s syllabus for the California Psychological Inventory. In P. McReynolds (Ed.), Advances in psychological assessment (Vol. 1). Palo Alto, CA: Science and Behavior Books.Google Scholar
  53. Green, Celia. (1968). Lucid dreams. London: Hamish Hamilton.Google Scholar
  54. Greyson, B. (1982). Near-death, out-of-body and lucid experiences: Additional comments and data. Lucidity Letter, 1(3), 6.Google Scholar
  55. Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (1974). Handedness, Sex and eyedness as moderating variables in the relation between hypnotic susceptibility and functional brain asymmetry. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83, 635–643.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Gur, R. C., & Gur, R. E. (1980). Handedness and individual differences in hemispheric activation. In J. Herron (Ed.), Neuropsychology of left handedness (pp. 211–231 ). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  57. Hakstain, A. R., & Cattel, R. B. (1975). An examination of adolescent sex differences in same ability and personality. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 1(4), 295–312.Google Scholar
  58. Harris, L. J. (1978). Sex differences in spatial ability: Possible environmental, genetic, and neurological factors. In M. Kinsbourne (Ed.), Asymmetrical function of the brain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  59. Hearne, K. M. T. (1978). Lucid dreams: An electrophysiological and psychological study. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Liverpool.Google Scholar
  60. Hearne, K. M. T. (1983). Features of lucid dreams: Questionnaire data and content analyses (1). Journal of Lucid Dream Research, 1 (1), 3–20.Google Scholar
  61. Hendricks, M., Cartwright, R. D. (1978). Experiencing level in dreams: An individual difference variable. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 15 (3), 292–298.Google Scholar
  62. Hulfish, S. (1978). Relationship of role identification, self-esteem, and intelligence to sex differences in field independence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 835–842.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Irwin, H. J. (1985). Flight of mind: A psychological study of the out-of-body experience. Metuchen, NJ: The Scarecrow Press.Google Scholar
  64. Kinsbourne, M., & Hicks, R. E. (1978). Mapping cerebral functional space: Competition and collaboration in human performance. In M. Kinsbourne (Ed.), Asymmetrical function of the brain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  65. Kohr, R. L. (1980). A survey of psi experiences among members of a special population. The Journal of American Society for Psychic Research, 74, 295–311.Google Scholar
  66. Kohr, R. L. (1982). Near death experience and its relationship to psi and various altered states. Theta, 10, 50–53.Google Scholar
  67. Kueny, Sallie R. (1985). An examination of auditory cueing in REM sleep for the induction of lucid dreams. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Pacific Graduate Schol of Psychology.Google Scholar
  68. LaBerge, S. P. (1985). Lucid dreaming. Los Angeles: Jeremy Tarcher.Google Scholar
  69. LaBerge, S. P., Gackenbach, J. I. (1982). [Self-perception testing]. Unpublished raw data Leftcourt, H. M., & Telegdi, M. S. (1971). Perceived focus of control and dependence as predictors of cognitive activity. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 37, 5356.Google Scholar
  70. Ley, R. G. (1983). Cerebral laterality and imagery. In A. A. Sheikh (Ed.), Imagery: Current theory, research, and application (pp. 252–287 ). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  71. McCabe, B. F., & Ryu, J. H. (1979). Vestibular physiology in understanding the dizzy patient. Rochester, MN: American Academy of Otolaryngology.Google Scholar
  72. McCarley, R. W., Hobson, J. A. (1979). The form of dreams and the biology of sleep. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of dreams: Research, theories and applications. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
  73. Megargee, E. I. (1972). The California psychological inventory handbook. San Francisco: JosseyBass.Google Scholar
  74. Meskin, B. B., & Singer, J. L. (1974). Reflective thought, and laterality of eye movements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 64–71.Google Scholar
  75. Milner, B. (1965). Visually-guided maze learning in man. Neuropsychologia, 3, 317–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Newcombe, S., Russell, W. R. (1969). Disassociated visual, perceptual and spatial deficits in focal lesions of the right hemisphere. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, Psychiatry, 32, 73–81.Google Scholar
  77. Noppe, L. D., & Gallagher, J. M. (1977). A cognitive style approach to creative thought. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 8590.Google Scholar
  78. Nussdorf, G. E. (1975). Sex, sex-role stereotypes and sex-role orientation as factors in risk-taking behavior. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Fordham University.Google Scholar
  79. Ogilvie, R., Hunt, H., Kushniruk, A., Newman, J. (1983). Lucid dreams and the arousal continuum. Sleep Research, 12, 182.Google Scholar
  80. Ohrmacht, F. W., & McMorris, R. F. (1971). Creativity as a function of field independence and dogmatism. Journal of Psychology, 79, 165–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Owens, W., & Limber, J. (1983). Lateral eye movement as a measure of cognitive ability and style. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 56, 711719.Google Scholar
  82. Palmer, J. (1974). A community mail survey of psychic experiences. Research in Parapsychology, 3, 130–133.Google Scholar
  83. Reed, Henry. (1977). Meditation and lucid dreaming: A statistical relationship. Sundance Community Dream Journal, 2, 237–238.Google Scholar
  84. Richardson, A. (1969). Mental imagery. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  85. Rosenberg, E. S. (1976). Some psychological and biological relationships between masculinity and femininity and field dependence and field independence. Dissertation Abstracts International, 36, 5875B.Google Scholar
  86. Shepard, R. N. (1978). The mental image. American Psychologist, 32, (2), 125–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Siegel, R. K. (1977). Hallucinations. Scientific American, 237, 132–140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Snyder, M. (1974). Self-monitoring of expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 526–537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Snyder, T. J., Gackenbach, J. I. (1981). Lucid dreaming and cerebral organization Sleep Research, 10, 154.Google Scholar
  90. Spence, J. T., Helmreich, R. L. (1978). Masculinity and femininity: Their psychological dimensions, correlates and antecedents. Austin and London: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  91. Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R., Stapp, J. (1975). Ratings of self and peers on sex role attributes and their relation to self-esteem and conceptions of masculinity and femininity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 568–571.Google Scholar
  92. Sporros, N. P., Stam, H. J., Radtke, H. D., Nightingale, M. E. (1980). Absorption in imagining, sex-role orientation, and the recall of dreams by males and females. Journal of Personality Assessment, 44, 227–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Spotts, J. V., & Mackler, B. (1976). Relationship of field-dependent and field-independent cognitive styles to creative test performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 24, 239–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Stoner, J. A. F. (1961). A comparison of individual and group decisions involving risk. Unpublished master’s thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Sloan School of Management.Google Scholar
  95. Tart, C. (1972). Altered states of consciousness. Garden City, New York: Anchor Books. Tart, C. (1975). States of consciousness. New York: E. P. Dutton.Google Scholar
  96. Torrance, E. P. (1972). Predictive validity of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Journal of Creative Behavior, 6(4), 236–252.Google Scholar
  97. Vandenberg, S., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations: A group test of three dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Van Nuys, D. W. (1984). Lateral eye movement and dream recall II: Sex differences and handedness. International Journal of Psychosomatics, 31(3), 3–7.Google Scholar
  99. Witkin, H. A., Goodenough, D. R. (1981). Cognitive styles: Essence and origins. New York:]International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  100. Witkin, H. A., Dyk. R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., Karp, S. A. (1962). Psychological differentiation: Studies in development. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. Witkin, H. A., & Goodenough, D. R., Oltman, P. K. (1979). Psychological differentiation: Current status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1127–1145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  103. Zuckerman, M., Bone, R. N., Neary, R., Mangelsdorff, D., Bustman, B. (1972). What is the sensation seeker? Personality trait and experience correlates of the sensation-seeking scales. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 39 (2), 308–321.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas J. Snyder
    • 1
  • Jayne Gackenbach
    • 2
  1. 1.Developmental Disabilities CenterUniversity of AlbertaEdmontonCanada
  2. 2.Department of PsychologyUniversity of Northern IowaCedar FallsUSA

Personalised recommendations