Patterns of Cardiovascular-Somatic-Respiratory Interactions in the Continuous Perceptual-Motor Task Paradigm

  • Sven Svebak
Part of the NATO ASI Series book series (NSSA, volume 114)


This chapter summarizes a series of psychophysiological studies conducted in our laboratory over a number of years. This research has been primarily concerned with physiological correlates of extrinsic and intrinsic motivational factors during task performance. Extrinsic factors have centered around task difficulty and threat of aversive stimulation during the performance of continuous perceptual-motor tasks. Intrinsic motivational factors of importance in our experiments have been related to individual differences in propensity to seriousmindedness and type A behavior. Because we have been interested in patterns of motivationally dependent task responses across a range of physiological systems, we have simultaneously measured cardiovascular, respiratory and neuromuscular parameters in our studies. Our findings indicate that intrinsic and extrinsic motivational factors interact in specific ways under different conditions to produce physiological patterns of reactivity to continuous perceptual-motor tasks.


Task Performance Task Difficulty Verbal Report Error Score Playful State 


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Apter, M. J., 1982, “The Experience of Motivation: The Theory of Psychological Reversals”, Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
  2. Brunia, C. H. M., 1983, Motor preparation changes in amplitude of achilles tendon reflexes during a fixed foreperiod of one second, Psycho-physiology, 20:658–664.Google Scholar
  3. Chesney, M. A., Eagleston, J. R., and Rosenman, R. H., 1981, Type A behavior: Assessment and intervention, in: “Medical Psychology: Contributions to Behavioral Medicine”, C. K. Prokop and L. A. Bradley, eds., pp. 19–36, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  4. Grossman, P., and Wientjes, K., 1985, Respiratory-cardiac coordination as an index of cardiac functioning, in: “Cardiovascular Psycho-physiology: Theory and Methods”, J. Orlbeke, G. Mulder and L. van Doornen, eds., pp. 451–464, Plenum Press, New York.Google Scholar
  5. Light, K. C., 1981, Cardiovascular responses to effortful active coping: Implications for the role of stress in hypertension development, Psychophysiology, 18:216–225.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Loveless, N. E., 1983, Event-related brain potentials and human performance, in: “Physiological Correlates of Human Behavior, Vol 2: Attention and Performance”, pp. 79–97, Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
  7. Malmo, R. B., 1965, “On Emotions Needs and our Archaic Brain”, Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York.Google Scholar
  8. Mulder, G., and Mulder, L. J. M., 1980, Coping with mental workload, in: “Coping and Health”, S. Levine and H. Ursin, eds., pp. 233–258, Plenum Press, New York.Google Scholar
  9. Murgatroyd, S., Rushton, C., Apter, M. J., and Ray, C., 1978, The development of the Telic Dominance Scale, J. Pers. Ass., 42:519–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Obrist, P. A., 1976, The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction — As it appears today, Psychophysiology, 13:95–107.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Obrist, P. A., Webb, R. A., Sutterer, J. R., and Howard, J. L., 1970, The cardiac-somatic relationship: Some reformulations, Psychophysiology, 6:569–587.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Pribram, K. H., and McGuinness, D., 1975, Arousal, activation and effort in the control of attention, Psychol. Rev., 82:116–149.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Rimehaug, T., and Svebak, S., 1984, Right and left upper arm flexor and extensor EMG-activity during cognitive-perceptual task performance: The significance of telic state and hedonic tone, Bull. Br. Psychol. Ass., 37:A46.Google Scholar
  14. Strømme, S. B., Wikeby, P. C., Blix, A. S., and Ursin, H., 1978, Additional heart rate, in: “Psychobiology of Stress: A Study of Coping Men”, H. Ursin, E. Baade and S. Levine, eds., pp. 83–89, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  15. Svebak, S., 1982, The effect of task difficulty and threat of aversive electric shock upon tonic physiological changes, Biol. Psychol., 14:113–128.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Svebak, S., 1983, The effect of information load, emotional load and motivational state upon tonic physiological activation, in: “Biological and Psychological Basis of Psychosomatic Disease”, H. Ursin and R. Murison, eds., pp. 61–73, Pergamon Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
  17. Svebak, S., 1984, Active forearm flexor tension patterns in the continuous perceptual-motor task paradigm: The significance of motivation, Internat. J. Psychophysiol., 2:167–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Svebak, S., Cardiac and somatic activation in the continuous perceptual- motor task: The significance of threat and serious-mindedness, Internat. J. Psychophysiol., 3:155–162.Google Scholar
  19. Svebak, S., and Apter, M. J., 1984, Type A behavior and its relation to seriousmindedness (Telic dominance), Scand. J. Psychol., 25:161–167.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Svebak, S., Dalen, K., and Storfjell, O., 1981, The psychological significance of task-induced tonic changes in somatic and autonomic activity, Psychophysiology, 18:403–409.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Svebak, S., and Murgatroyd, S., 1985, Telic dominance: A multimethod validation of reversal theory constructs., J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 48:107–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Svebak, S., Storfjell, O., and Dalen, K., 1982, The effect of a threatening context upon motivation and task-induced physiological changes, Br. J. Psychol., 73:505–512.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Ursin, H., Murison, R., and Knardahl, S., 1983, Conclusion: Sustained activation and disease, in: “Biological and psychological basis of psychosomatic disease”, H. Ursin and R. Murison, eds., pp. 269–277, Pergamon Press, Oxford.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1986

Authors and Affiliations

  • Sven Svebak
    • 1
  1. 1.University of BergenNorway

Personalised recommendations