Skip to main content

Behavioral Foundations of Adaptation

  • Chapter
Perspectives in Ethology

Abstract

Maintenance in the face of change is the status quo for most organisms. Remaining adapted requires that behavior be coupled with changes in environments, which may be extremely complex in their patterning or essentially unpredictable; we understand only superficially the processes through which coupling is established and maintained. Here I explore this problem by considering the paradox that animals are both conservative in their behavior, or resilient with regard to changes in their surroundings, and able to track these changes by plastically altering their own behavior. Consideration of these qualities, especially how they interact in the context of various kinds of environments and various patterns of environmental variation, yields useful insights into adaptation and raises fundamental questions about the organization of behaving systems in general.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Astrom, K. J. (1980). Self-tuning regulators—Design principles and applications. In Narendra, K. S., and Monopoli, R. V. (eds.), Applications of Adaptive Control, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, D. E. (1977). Cellular Energy Metabolism and Its Regulation, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandler, R., and Grinder, J. (1975). The Structure of Magic. I: A Book about Language and Therapy, Science and Behavior Books, Palo Alto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and Nature, Dutton, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beer, S. (1975). Platform for Change, Wiley, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohm, D. (1969). Some remarks on the notion of order. In Waddington, C. H. (ed.), Towards and Theoretical Biology, Aldine Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonner, J. (1973). Control programs in biological development. In Pattee, H. H. (ed.), Hierarchy Theory: The Challenge of Complex Systems, George Braziller, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., Franks, J. J., Nitsch, K. E. and McCarrell, N. S. (1977). Toward unex-plaining memory. In Shaw, R. S., and Bransford, J. D. (eds.), Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, W. F. (1973). The problem of meaning and the interrelationship of the higher mental processes. In Weimer, W. B., and Palermo, D. S. (eds.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronowski, J. (1977). A Sense of the Future, MIT Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronowski, J. (1978). The Origins of Knowledge and Imagination, Yale University Press, New Haven.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browne, J. C. (1983). Parallel computations and the next generation of supercomputers. SIAM News 16:8–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calder, W. A. (1974). Consequences of body size for avian energetics. In Paynter, R. A., Jr. (ed.), Avian Energetics, Nuttall Ornithology Club Publ. #15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calvert, T. W., Chapman, J., and Patla, A. (1982). The simulation of human movement. Proc. Graphics Interface 82:227–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cerf, V. G., and Kirstein, P. K. (1978). Issues in packet-network interconnection. Proc. IEEE 66:1386–1408.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, MIT Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cyert, R. M. (1983). Personal computing in education and research. Science 222:1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). Hierarchical organization: A candidate principle for ethology. In Bateson, P. P. G., and Hinde, R. A. (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edelman, G. M. (1978). Group selection and phasic reentrant signaling: A theory of higher brain function. In Edelman, G. M., and Mountcastle, V. B., (eds.), The Mindful Brain, MIT Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Findley, D. (1981). Applied Time Series Analysis, Vol. 2, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, J. P., Cowan, I. McT. and Holling, C. S. (1977). Prey capture by the African lion. Can. J. Zool. 55:1811–1828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feinsinger, P., and Colwell, R. K. (1978). Community organization among neotropical nectar-feeding birds. Am. Zool. 18:779–795.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fentress, J. C. (1972). Development and patterning of movement sequences in inbred mice. In Kiger, J. (ed.), The Biology of Behavior, Oregon State University Press, Corvallis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fentress, J. C. (1973). Specific and nonspecific factors in the causation of behavior. In Bateson, P. P. G., and Klopfer, P. H. (eds.), Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 1, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fentress, J. C. (1976). Dynamic boundaries of patterned behaviour: Interaction and self-organization. In Bateson, P. P. G., and Hinde, R. A. (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fentress, J. C., and Stilwell, F. (1973). Grammar of a movement sequence in inbred mice. Nature 244:52–53.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1962). Cognitive dissonance. Sci. Am. 207:93–102.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fretwell, S. D. (1972). Populations in a Seasonal Environment, Princeton University Press, Princeton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromm, E. (1965). Escape from Freedom, Avon, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel, C. R. 1980. The Organization of Action: A New Synthesis, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gass, C. L. (1978). Experimental studies of foraging in complex laboratory environments. Am. Zool. 18:617–626.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gass, C. L., and Montgomerie, R. D. (1981). Hummingbird foraging behaviour: Decisionmaking and energy regulation. In Kamil, A. C., and Sargent, T. D. (eds.), Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches, Garland, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gendlin, E. T. (1978). Focusing, Everest House, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems, Houghton-Mifflin, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giuliano, V. E. (1982). The mechanism of office work. Sci. Am. 247:114–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golani, I. (1976). Homeostatic motor processes in mammalian interactions: A choreography of display. In Bateson, P. P. G., and Klopfer, P. H. (eds.), Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 2, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, D., and Kelso, J. A. S. (1980). Are movements prepared in parts? Not under compatible (naturalized) conditions. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 109:475–495.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, D. R. (1981). The Question of Animal Awareness, William Kaufman, Los Altos.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grobstein, C. (1973). Hierarchical order and neogenesis. In Pattee, H. H. (ed.), Hierarchy Theory: The Challenge of Complex Systems, Braziller, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groves, S. (1982). Aspects of foraging in Black Oystercatchers, Ph.D. Thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver (unpublished).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunn, T. G. (1982). The mechanization of design and manufacturing. Sci. Am. 247:114–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hailman, J. P. (1982). Ontogeny: Toward a general theoretical framework for ethology. In Bateson, P. P. G., and Klopfer, P. H. (eds.), Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 5, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hainsworth, F. R., and Wolf, L. L. (1978). The economics of temperature regulation in non-mammalian organisms. In Wang, L., and Hudson, J., (eds.), Strategies in the Cold: Natural Torpidity and Thermogenesis, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halwes, T. (1973). Structural realism, coalitions, and the relationship of Gisbonian, Con-structivist, and Buddhist theories of perception. In Weimer, W. B., and Palermo, D. S. (eds.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes, Lawrence Earlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hixon, M. A., Carpenter, F. L., and Paton, D. C. (1983). Territory area, flower density, and time budgeting in hummingbirds: An experimental and theoretical analysis. Am. Nat. 122:366–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofstadter, D. R. (1979). Goedel, Escher, Bach: An Eternal Golden Braid, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holling, C. S. (1978). Adaptive Environmental Assessment and Management, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingber, L. (1981). Attention, physics, and teaching. J. Soc. Biol. Struct. 4:225–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janetos, A. C., and Cole, B. J. (1981). Imperfectly optimal animals. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 9:203–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jantsch, E. (1980). The Self-Organizing Universe: Scientific and Human Implications of the Emerging Paradigm of Evolution, Pergamon Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. J. (1977). Remember that old theory of memory? Well forget it!! In Shaw, R. S., and Bransford, J. D. (eds.), Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, Lawrence-Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., and Sargent, T. D. (1981). Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches, Garland, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kamil, A. C., and Yoerg, S. I. (1982). Learning and foraging behavior. In Bateson, P. P. G., and Klopfer, P. H. (eds.), Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 5, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W. (1973). Attention and Human Performance, Goodyear, Pacific Palisades.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keele, S. W. (1980). Behavioral analysis of motor control. In Brooks, V. B. (ed.), Handbook of Physiology (Vol. 3): Motor Control, American Physiology Society, Bethesda.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., and Tuller, B. (1981). Toward a theory of apractic syndromes. Brain Language 12:224–245.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Laing, R. D. (1967). The Politics of Experience, Penguin, Markham.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laing, R. D. (1972). The Politics of the Family and Other Essays, Vintage, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levine, R. D. (1982). Supercomputers. Sci. Am. 246:118–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levins, R. (1973). The limits of complexity. In Pattee, H. H. (ed.), Hierarchy Theory: The Challenge of Complex Systems, George Braziller, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, D. I. (1983). Carnegie-Mellon and IBM sign joint development contract. SigPC Notes 5(1/2):6–8.

    Google Scholar 

  • March, J. G. (1978). Bounded rationality, ambiguity, and the engineering of choice. Bell. J. Economics 9:587–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marier, P. (1979). Early patterns of perception. Nat. Hist. 88:7–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R. (1978). Biology of language: The epistemology of reality. In Miller, G. A., and Lenneberg, E. (eds.), Psychology and Biology of Language and Thought: Essays in Honor of Eric Lenneberg, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R. (1980). Autopoiesis: Reproduction, heredity, and evolution. In Zeleny, M. (ed.), Autopoiesis: Dissipative Structures and Spontaneous Social Order, Westview Press, Boulder.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R., and Guiloff, G. D. (1980). The quest for the intelligence of intelligence. J. Soc. Biol. Struct. 3:135–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R., and Varela, F. J. (1972). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living, D. Reidel, Dordrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • May, R. M. (1976). Theoretical Ecology: Principles and Applications, W. B. Saunders, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. 1978. Optimization theory in evolution. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 9:31–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1974). Behavior programs and evolutionary strategies. Am. Sci. 62:650–659.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Menzel, E. W., and Juno, C. (1982). Marmosets (Saguinus fuscicollis): Are learning sets learned? Science 217:750–752.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Menzel, E. W., and Wyers, E. J. (1981). Cognitive aspects of animal behaviour. In Kamil, A. C. and Sargent, T. D. (eds.), Foraging Behaviour: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches, Garland, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 63:81–97.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomerie, R. D., and Gass, C. L. (1981). Energy limitation of hummingbird populations in tropical and temperate communities. Oecologia 50:162–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moskowitz, B. A. (1978). The acquisition of language. Sci. Am. 239:92–108.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Newman, W. M., and Sproull, R. F. (1979). Principles of Interactive Computer Graphics, 2nd ed. McGraw-Hill, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pattee, H. H. (1973). The physical basis and origin of hierarchical control. In Pattee, H. H. (ed.), Hierarchy Theory: The Challenge of Complex Systems, Braziller, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pattee, H. H. (1982). The need for complementarity in models of cognitive behaviour: A response to Fowler and Turvey. In Weimer, W. B., and Palermo, D. S. (eds.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes, Vol. 2, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patton, G. S. (1947). War As I Knew It, Part 3, Houghton-Mifflin, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, K. H. (1982). Reflections on the place of brain in the ecology of mind. In Weimer, W. B., and Palermo, D. S. (eds.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes, Vol. 2, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulliam, H. R. (1981). Learning to forage optimally. In Kamil, A. C., and Sargent, T. D. (eds.), Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches, Garland, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulliam, H. R., and Dunford, C. (1980). Programmed to Learn, Columbia University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pyke, G. H., Pulliam, H. R., and Charnov, E. L. (1977). Optimal foraging: A selective review of theory and tests. Q. Rev. Biol. 52:137–154.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., Battalio, R., Kagel, J., and Green, L. (1981). Maximization theory in behavioural psychology. Behav. Brain Sci. 4:371–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruitenbeek, H. M. (1972). Going Crazy: The Radical Therapy of R. D. Laing and Others, Bantam, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schänk, R. C., and Abelson, R. P. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol. Rev. 84:1–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, R. S., and Bransford, J. D. (1977). Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, R. S., and Turvey, M. T. (1981). Coalitions as models for ecosystems: A realist perspective on perceptual organization. In Kubovy, M., and Pomerantz, J. R. (eds.), Perceptual Organization, Lawrence Erlbaum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, R. S., Turvey, M. T., and Mace, W. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequences of a commitment to realism. In Weimer, W. B., and Palermo, D. S. (eds.), Cognition and the Symbolic Processes, Vol. 2, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. (1971). Constraints on learning. Adv. Study Anim. Behav. 4:1–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shettleworth, S. J. (1984). Function and mechanism in learning. In Zeiler, M., and Harzern, P. (eds.), Advances in analysis of behavior, Vol. 3: Biological Factors in Learning, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., and Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychol. Rev. 84:127–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shoch, J. F., Dalai, Y. K., Crane, R. C., and Redell, D. D. (1981). Evolution of the Ethernet Local Computer Network, Xerox Office Products Division T8102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1973). The organization of complex systems. In Pattee, H. H. (ed.), Hierarchy Theory: The Challenge of Complex Systems, Braziller, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1974). How big is a chunk? Science 183:482–488.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1976). The information-processing system called ‘human memory.’ In Rosenzweig, M. R. and Bennett, E. L. (eds.), Neural Mechanisms of Learning and Memory, MIT Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1978). On how to decide what to decide. Bell J. Economics 9:494–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H.A. (1979a). Rational decision-making in business organizations. In Les Prix Nobel, 1978, Almquist and Wiksell, Stockholm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1979b). Information processing models of cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 30:363–396.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond Freedom and Dignity, Knopf, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science 213:501–504.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Slovic, P., Kunreuther, H., and White, G. F. (1974). Decision processes, rationality and adjustment to natural hazards. In White, G. F. (ed.), Natural Hazards: Local, National, Global, Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snodgrass, R. (1980). Proposal for a joint effort in personal scientific computing: Carnegie-Mellon University. SigPC Notes 3(3/4):40–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staddon, J. E. R. (1980). Limits to Action: The Allocation of Individual Behaviour, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sutherland, G. D. (1984). Spatial Memory Processes in Hummingbirds: The Influence of Energetics and Spatial Distribution of Food Resources on Foraging Behaviour. M. Sc. Thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver (unpublished).

    Google Scholar 

  • Turvey, M. T. (1977). Preliminaries to a theory of action with reference to vision. In Shaw, R. S., and Bransford, J. D. (eds.), Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walters, C. J., and Hilborn, R. (1978). Ecological optimization and adaptive management. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 9:157–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weimer, W. B. (1977). Motor theories of the mind. In Shaw, R. S., Bransford, J. D. (eds.), Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1979). Chunking and consolidation: A theoretical synthesis of semantic networks, configuring in conditioning, S-R versus cognitive learning, normal forgetting, the amnesic syndrome, and the hippocampal arousal system. Psychol. Rev. 86:44–60.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, W. A. (1981). Human learning and memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 32:21–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wickramasekera, I. (1976). Biofeedback, Behavior Therapy, and Hypnosis: Potentiating the Verbal Control of Behaviour for Clinicians, Nelson-Hall, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, L. L., and Hainsworth, F. R., and Gill, F. B. (1975). Foraging efficiencies and time budgets in nectar-feeding birds. Ecology 56:117–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, L. L., Stiles, F. G., and Hainsworth, F. R. (1976). Ecological organization of a tropical, highland hummingbird community. J. Anim. Ecol. 45:349–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zach, R. (1978). Selection and dropping of whelks by Northwestern crows. Behaviour 67:134–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zach, R., and Smith, J. N. M. (1981). Optimal foraging in wild birds? In Kamil, A. C., and Sargent, T. D. (eds.), Foraging Behavior: Ecological, Ethological, and Psychological Approaches, Garland, New York.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1985 Plenum Press, New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gass, C.L. (1985). Behavioral Foundations of Adaptation. In: Bateson, P.P.G., Klopfer, P.H. (eds) Perspectives in Ethology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-0232-3_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-0232-3_4

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4757-0234-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-4757-0232-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics