Advertisement

On the Relation between Language Comprehension and Language Production in a Social Psychological Perspective

  • Bente Eriksen Hagtvet

Abstract

The assumption that language comprehension is developmentally ahead of language production has traditionally been considered an indisputable fact (Brown 1973, Chomsky 1964, Ingram 1974, Lenneberg 1962, McCarthy 1954). This position has received considerable experimental support (Cinque 1973, Cocking 1977, Fraser et al. 1963, Lovell & Dixon 1967, Nelson 1977, Nurss & Day 1971). Many investigators have, however, more recently taken other positions, e.g. that comprehension equals production (Fernald 1972), that production precedes comprehension (Chapman & Miller 1973, Clark 1974), or that the relationship shifts and varies with age, situation and experience (Bloom 1974).

Keywords

Target Object Language Comprehension Language Production Poor Comprehenders Object Context 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Blakar R.M. og Rommetveit, R., 1975, Utterances in vacuo and in contexts, International J. Psycholinguistics, 4, 5–32Google Scholar
  2. Bloom L., 1974, Talking, understanding and thinking, In “Acquisition Retardation and Intervention” R.L. Schiefelbusch & L.L. Lloyd ed., Macmillan, London.Google Scholar
  3. Brown R., 1973, A first language: the early stages, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass.Google Scholar
  4. Champman R. and Miller J., 1973, Word order in early two and three word utterances: Does production precede comprehension? Paper presented to the Fifth Annual Child Language Research Forum, Stanford University, April 7.Google Scholar
  5. Chomsky N., 1964, Formal discussion of Wick Miller and Susan Ervin-the development of grammar in child language, In “The acquisition of language” U. Bellugi & R. Brown ed. Monogr. Soc. res. Ch. Develop, 29, 35–70Google Scholar
  6. Cinque J.A., 1973, Imitation, comprehension and production of morphological language constructs within subjects in their native and second language; Dissertation Abstracts International, 34 (5-A), p. 2381Google Scholar
  7. Clark R., Hutchinson S., Van Buren P., 1974, Comprehension and production in language acquisition, Journal of Linguistics 10, 39–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cocking R.R., 1977, Measures of language comprehension: relations to performance; Child Study Journal, 7, 165–178Google Scholar
  9. Fernald C., 1972, Control of Grammar in imitation, comprehension and production: Problems of replication; J. Verb. Learning Verb. Behav. 11, 606–613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fraser C., Bellugi U. & Brown R., 1963, Control of grammar in imitation, comprehension and production, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 2, 121–135CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Hagtvet B.E., 1980a, Does comprehension precede production? Unpublished manuscript. The Norwegian Post graduate teacher training center for special education, OsloGoogle Scholar
  12. Hagtvet B.E., 1980b, Språk, kommunikasjon og utvikling (Language, Communication and development), Universitetsforlatet, Oslo.Google Scholar
  13. Ingram D., 1974, The relationship between comprehension and production;In “Acquisition, Retardation and Intervention” R.L. Schiefelbusch & L.L.Lloyd ed., Macmillan, LondonGoogle Scholar
  14. Krauss R.M. & Glucksberg S., 1969, The development of Communication Competence as a function of age; Child Development, 40, 255–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Lenneberg E.H., 1962, Understanding language without ability to speak: A case report; J. Abnorm Soc. Psych, 65, 419–425CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Lovell K. & Dixon E.M., 1967, The growth of the control of grammar in imitation, comprehension and production; Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 31–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. McCarthy D., 1954, Language development. In “Manual of Child Psychology” L. Carmichael ed., Wiley N.Y.Google Scholar
  18. Nelson K., 1977, First steps in Language Acquisition, J. of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 16, 563–583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Nurss J.R. & Day D.E., 1971, Imitation, Comprehension and Production of Grammatical Structures, J.Verb, learning Verb. Behav. 10, 68–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Olson D.R., 1966, On Conceptual Strategies, In “Studies in Cognitive Growth” J. Bruner ed., Wiley, N.Y., pp. 135–154Google Scholar
  21. Olson D.R., 1970, Language and Thought: Aspects of a cognitive theory of semantics; Psychological Review, 77, pp. 257–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Piaget J., 1926, The Language and Thought of the Child, Routledge & Kagan Paul, LondonGoogle Scholar
  23. Rommetveit R., 1968: Words, Meanings and Messages, Academic Press, N.Y.Google Scholar
  24. Rommetveit R., 1972, Språk, tanke og kommunikasjon (Language, thought and communication); Univ. forl., OsloGoogle Scholar
  25. Rommetveit R., 1974, On Message Structure, Wiley, LondonGoogle Scholar
  26. Rommetveit R., 1977, On Piagetian Cognitive Operations, semantic competence and message structure in adult-child communication; In “The Social Context of Language”, I. Markova ed., Wiley, LondonGoogle Scholar
  27. Rommetveit R., 1980, Ordinary Language as “a Form of Life” Unpublished Manuscript, University of Oslo.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Bente Eriksen Hagtvet
    • 1
  1. 1.Training Center of Special EducationOsloNorway

Personalised recommendations