Advertisement

Perception and Performance of Musical Rhythm

  • Alf Gabrielsson

Abstract

Rhythm is often said to be the most fundamental element of music in any culture of the world (see, for instance, Gaston, 1968). In spite of this there is still very much confusion about the concept of rhythm, both on the empirical and on the theoretical side. Many rhythm phenomena have proved to be elusive for analytical and/or empirical approaches, and the attempts at theoretical explanations are rather limited in number as well as in scope. Most of the experimental research on rhythm does not directly refer to rhythm in connection with music — there are some outstanding exceptions, however. For historical reviews of rhythm research see Gabrielsson (1973a, 1979).

Keywords

Sound Event Musical Training Mechanical Norm Motion Character Music Theory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bengtsson, I., 1974, Empirische Rhythmusforschung in Uppsala, Hamburger Jahrbuch für Musikwissenschaft, 1:195–220.Google Scholar
  2. Bengtsson, I., and Gabrielsson, A., 1977, Rhythm research in Uppsala, In “Music. Room and Acoustics,” Royal Swedish Academy of Music, Stockholm.Google Scholar
  3. Bengtsson, I., and Gabrielsson, A., 1980, Methods for analyzing performance of musical rhythm, Scand. J. Psychol., 21 (in press).Google Scholar
  4. Bengtsson, I., and Gabrielsson, A., and Gabrielsson, B., 1978, RHYTHM-SYVARD, A computer program for analysis of rhythmic performance, Swedish. J, Musicoh, 60:15–24.Google Scholar
  5. Bengtsson, I., Gabrielsson, A., and Thorsen, S.M 1969, Empiriskrytmforskning (Empirical rhythm research), Swedish. J. Musicol., 31:49–118.Google Scholar
  6. Bengtsson, I., Tove, P.A., and Thorsen, S.M., 1972, Sound analysis equipment and rhythm research ideas at the Institue of Musicology in Uppsala, Studm instrumentorum musicae popularis, 2:33–76.Google Scholar
  7. Fraisse, P., 1956, “Les structures rytnmiques,” Publications Universitaires de Louvain, Louvain.Google Scholar
  8. Fraisse, P., 1978, Time and rhythm perception, in “Handbook of Perception,” Vol 8, E.G. Carterette and M.P. Friedman, eds., Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  9. Gabrielsson, A., 1973a, Similarity ratings and dimension analyses of auditory rhythm patterns, I, Scand. J. Psychol., 14:138–160.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gabrielsson, A., 1973b, Similarity ratings and dimension analyses of auditory rhythm patterns, II, Scand. J. Psychol., 14:161–176.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Gabrielsson, A., 1973c, Adjective ratings and dimension analyses of auditory rhythm patterns, Scand. J. Psychol., 14: 244–260.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gabrielsson, A., 1973c, Studies in rhythm. Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis: Abstracts of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences, 7.Google Scholar
  13. Gabrielsson, A., 1974a, Performance of rhythm patterns, Scand. J. Psychol., 13:63–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gabrielsson, A., 1974b, An empirical comparison between some models for multidimensional scaling, Scand. J. Psychol., 15:73–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gabrielsson, A., 1979, Experimental research on rhythm. Human. Assoc. Rev., 30:69–92.Google Scholar
  16. Gaston, E.T., 1968, Man and music, In “Music in Therapy,” E.T. Gaston ed., Macmillan, New York.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1982

Authors and Affiliations

  • Alf Gabrielsson
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyUppsala UniversityUppsalaSweden

Personalised recommendations