The Fixed Action Pattern: Empirical Properties and Theoretical Implications

  • Howard Moltz

Abstract

In a recent article, Lorenz [1, p. 53] wrote, “Like genetics and many other branches of inductive science, ethology started from a real discovery and definitely not from a theory. This is a fact that we do not want to be forgotten.” The discovery to which Lorenz referred was made independently at the beginning of the present century by two zoologists, Charles Whitman and Oskar Heinroth. What they discovered was that certain behavior patterns are so typical of particular taxonomic groups as to provide reliable criteria for systematic classification—as reliable, in most cases, as anatomical structures. Behavior patterns having the same or similar properties were subsequently identified in a number of vertebrate and invertebrate forms and were designated as “instinctive movements” (Erbkoordinationen) by Lorenz and as “fixed actions patterns” (FAPs) by most other ethologists.

Keywords

Maternal Behavior Directed Head Appetitive Behavior Empirical Property Graylag Goose 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. 1.
    Lorenz, K.: The objective theory of instinct. in L’Instinct dans le Comportement des Animaux et de l’Homme, Masson et Cie, Paris, 1956.Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Baerends, G. P.: The ethological analysis of incubation behaviour, Ibis 101: 357, 1959.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Tinbergen, N.: The Study of Instinct, Oxford Univ. Oxford Press, 1951.Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Baerends, G. P.: The ethological analysis of fish behavior. in Brown, M. E., Ed.: The Physiology of Fishes, Vol. I I, Academic Press, New York, 1957.Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Dane, B., Walcott, C., and Drury, W. H.: The form andduration of the display actions of the goldeneye (Bucephala clangula) Behaviour 14: 265, 1959.Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Morris, P.: ‘Typical intensity’ and its relation to the problem of ritualisation, ibid. 11: 1, 1957.Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Tinbergen, N. Comparative study of the behaviour of gulls (Laridae): A progress report, Behaviour 15: 1, 1959.Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Lorenz, K.: Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels, J. Orn. Lpz. 83:137, 289, 1935. in Schiller, C., Ed. Instinctive Behavior, International Univ. Press, New York, 1957.Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Lorenz, K.: Ueber die Bildung des Instinktbegriffes, Naturwissenschaften 25:289, 307, 324, 1937. in Schiller, C., Ed.: Instinctive Behavior, International Univ. Press, New York, 1957.Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Lorenz, K.: Vergleichende Bewegungsstudien an Anatinen, J. Ornithol. 89: Sonderheft 19, 194, 1941.Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    Lorenz, K.: The evolution of behavior, Sci. Am. 199 (6): 67, 1958.Google Scholar
  12. 12.
    Eihl-Eibesfeldt, L, and Kramer, S.: Ethology, the comparative study of animal behavior, Quart. Rev. Biol. 33: 181, 1958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Lorenz, K., and Tinbergen, N.: Taxis und Instinkthandlung in der Eirollbewegung der Graugans, Z. Tierpsychol. 2:1, 1938. in Schiller, C., Ed.: Instinctive Behavior, International Univer. Press, New York, 1957.Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Prechtl, H. F. R.: The directed head turning response and allied movements of the human baby, Behaviour 13: 212, 1958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Baerends, D. P., and Baerends-van Roon, J. M.: An introduction to the study of the ethology of cichlid fishes, ibid., suppl. I, 1950.Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Hinde, R. A.: Some recent trands in ethology. in Koch, S., Ed.: Psychology: A Study of a Science. Vol. I I. McGraw-Hill, New York, 1959.Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Iersel, J. J. A. van: An analysis of the parental behaviour of the male threespined stickleback, Behaviour, Suppl. I II, 1953.Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Lorenz, K.: Vergleichende Verhaltenforschung, Zool. Anz. Suppl. 12:69, 1939. in Schiller, C., Ed.: Instinctive Behavior, International Univ. Press, New York, 1957.Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Lorenz, K.: Morphology and behavior patterns in closely allied species. in Schaffner, B., Ed.: Group Processes, Josiah Macy, Fund. 1955.Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Thorpe, W. H.: Some concepts of ethology, Nature 174: 101, 1954.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Tinbergen, N.: An objectivistic study of the innate behaviour of animals, Biblioth. biotheor. 1: 39, 1942.Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Cullen, E.: Experiment on the effect of social isolation on reproductive behaviour in the three-spined stickleback. Anim. Behay. 8:235, 1960. (Abs.)Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Grohmann, J.: Modifikation oder Funktionsreifung? Z. Tierpsychol. 2: 132, 1939.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. 24.
    Tinbergen, N., and Kuenen, D. J.: Ueber die ausloesenden und die richtunggebenden Reizsituationen der Sperrbewegung von jungen Drosseln (Turdus m. merula L und T.e. ericetorum Turton), ibid. 3:37, 1939. in Schiller, C., Ed.: Instinctive Behavior, International Univ. Press, New York, 1957.Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Lorenz, K.: The comparative method in studying innate behaviour patterns, Symp. Soc. Exp. Biol. 4: 221, 1950.Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Bullock, T. H.: The origins of patterned nervous discharge, Behaviour 17: 48, 1961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Miller, N. E.: Experiments on motivation: studies concerning psychological, physiological, and pharmacological techniques, Science 126: 1271, 1957.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Andersson, B., Jewell, P. A., and Larsson, S.: An appraisal of the effects of diencephalic stimulation of conscious animals in terms of normal behavior. Neurological Basis of Behaviour (Ciba Symposium), Churchill, London, 1958.Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Harris, G. W., Michael, R. P., and Schott, P. P.: Neurological site of stilbesterol in eliciting sexual behaviour, Neurological Basis of Behaviour (Ciba Symposium), Churchill, London, 1958.Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Adrian, E. D.: The control of nerve-cell activity, Symp. Soc. Exp. Biol. 4: 85, 1950.Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    Holst, E. von.: Studien ueber Reflexe und Rhythmen beim Goldfisch (Carassius auratus), Z. vergleich. Physiol. 20: 582, 1934.Google Scholar
  32. 32.
    Maynard, D. M.: Activity in a crustacean ganglion. II. Pattern and interaction in burst formation. Biol. Bull. 109: 420, 1955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. 33.
    Adrian, E. D., and Buytendijk, F. J. J.: Potential changes in the isolated brain of the goldfish, J. Physiol. (Brit.) 71: 121, 1931.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press Inc. 1962

Authors and Affiliations

  • Howard Moltz

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations